Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Validity evidence for a novel instrument assessing medical student attitudes toward instruction in implicit bias recognition and management

Abstract

Background

Implicit bias instruction is becoming more prevalent in health professions education, with calls for skills-based curricula moving from awareness and recognition to management of implicit bias. Evidence suggests that health professionals and students learning about implicit bias (“learners”) have varying attitudes about instruction in implicit bias, including the concept of implicit bias itself. Assessing learner attitudes could inform curriculum development and enable instructional designs that optimize learner engagement. To date, there are no instruments with evidence for construct validity that assess learner attitudes about implicit bias instruction and its relevance to clinical care.

Methods

The authors developed a novel instrument, the Attitude Towards Implicit Bias Instrument (ATIBI) and gathered evidence for three types of construct validity- content, internal consistency, and relationship to other variables.

Results

Authors utilized a modified Delphi technique with an interprofessional team of experts, as well as cognitive interviews with medical students leading to item refinement to improve content validity. Seven cohorts of medical students, N = 1072 completed the ATIBI. Psychometric analysis demonstrated high internal consistency (α = 0.90). Exploratory factor analysis resulted in five factors. Analysis of a subset of 100 medical students demonstrated a moderate correlation with similar instruments, the Integrative Medicine Attitude Questionnaire (r = 0.63, 95% CI: [0.59, 0.66]) and the Internal Motivation to Respond Without Prejudice Scale (r = 0.36, 95% CI: [0.32, 0.40]), providing evidence for convergent validity. Scores on our instrument had low correlation to the External Motivation to Respond Without Prejudice Scale (r = 0.15, 95% CI: [0.09, 0.19]) and the Groningen Reflection Ability Scale (r = 0.12, 95% CI: [0.06, 0.17]) providing evidence for discriminant validity. Analysis resulted in eighteen items in the final instrument; it is easy to administer, both on paper form and online.

Conclusion

The Attitudes Toward Implicit Bias Instrument is a novel instrument that produces reliable and valid scores and may be used to measure medical student attitudes related to implicit bias recognition and management, including attitudes toward acceptance of bias in oneself, implicit bias instruction, and its relevance to clinical care.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Patients worldwide continue to report prejudice and bias in their clinical encounters [1,2,3,4,5,6]. Evidence suggests that implicit bias may be contributing to these disparate clinical experiences [7]. Implicit bias refers to the unconscious, unintentional mental associations we make based on social identity groups; it is a result in part, of systemic discrimination [8]. It is most commonly measured by the Implicit Association Test, a publicly available response latency test that pairs images and value laden words [9]. Implicit bias contributes to health disparities through its influence on provider communication patterns and clinical decision-making [10]. This evidence spans the spectrum of training and practice and is relevant to all health professions [7, 11,12,13,14,15,16].

Due to its contributions to health disparities, implicit bias is a focus of instruction in health professions education. Curricula have been published in undergraduate, graduate, and continuing health professions education [17,18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25]. These interventions have increased knowledge and awareness of implicit bias, with some achieving strategy identification to address it [17, 20]. Recently, enthusiasm for moving implicit bias instruction from increased awareness to skill-development and practice has emerged [26]. This approach facilitates learners developing skills to manage their biases in order to optimize the outcomes of their clinical encounters, a process called implicit bias recognition and management (IBRM) [27]. The outcome of skills-based IBRM instruction is behavioral, rather than a change in the IAT score [27]. Focusing on behaviors as the outcome of IBRM instruction would obviate the limitations of the IAT [28].

Attitudes influence behaviors. Qualitative explorations of students’ attitudes have at times demonstrated resistance to the existence of implicit bias in general, its presence within oneself, its relevance to clinical care, and other aspects of instruction [17, 26, 29]. Physicians, nurses, and residents, struggle with reconciling their implicit bias with their professional identities [30, 31]. We have demonstrated the detrimental effect of learner resistance on faculty perceptions of their ability to facilitate implicit bias instruction [32]. Our previous work expanded on what is known about student resistance revealing potential threats to engagement with IBRM instruction, as well as opportunities to maximize engagement [26].

Better understanding attitudes about implicit bias could inform curriculum development. To our knowledge, no validated instrument exists to assess learner attitudes toward acceptance of bias in oneself, implicit bias instruction, or its relevance to clinical care (henceforth collectively referred to as IBRM instruction). Without a validated instrument to assess these attitudes, comprehensive curriculum development and program evaluation related to IBRM will remain an elusive goal. To address this gap, we designed and obtained evidence for construct validity of a novel instrument assessing student attitudes about IBRM instruction.

Methods

We developed the Attitude Toward Implicit Bias Instrument (ATIBI) through a series of steps to assess three types of validity: content, internal structure, and relationship to other variables [33]. We followed the instrument validation processes and methodology that have been established in related literature on attitude measurement in clinical education [34,35,36]. The study and all methods and procedures were reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of the Albert Einstein College of Medicine; it was deemed exempt research, no written consent was required.

Construct validity- content

Item design

The initial item design began with a literature search (CMG) of PubMed, ERIC, PsycNET, and Google Scholar using the terms “implicit bias” or “unconscious bias” or “subconscious bias” and “attitudes” to identify any existing instruments in the winter of 2015. No existing validated instruments were identified. We generated initial survey items among our team (CMG, RJG, PRM) informed by three sources: 1) Prior survey design at our institution [18]; 2) Initial qualitative data analysis of medical students’ perceptions of challenges and opportunities for participating in implicit bias instruction [26]; and 3) Lessons learned from our experiences delivering instruction on implicit bias [18, 20]. After two rounds of revisions, we convened a panel of experts to participate in a modified Delphi technique in the spring of 2015; the Delphi was considered modified as the group interacted during the two pre-determined rounds [37]. In the spring of 2015, experts included three cognitive psychologists, and four clinician-investigators (one nurse and three physicians), all with previous experience in implicit bias education. We sent the initial survey items out for review, with the option to provide written feedback prior to the initial meeting. The meeting was held via web conference, each item was reviewed, accepted, reworded, or discarded. Additional items were suggested. Items were sent out for another round of feedback. During the second and final meeting, we reviewed each item and achieved consensus through discussion. We also agreed to a six-item Likert-type scale with anchors ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree; we chose an even-numbered scale to avoid a neutral option, given our focus on learner attitudes about this topic.

Item refinement

In order to refine the final items on the scale, we recruited a convenience sample of 20 medical student volunteers from Albert Einstein College of Medicine in Bronx, NY, USA. Students completed the instrument and participated in cognitive interviews for each question in the summer of 2015. During the cognitive interviews, investigators read each survey item with the medical student volunteers and discussed their interpretation of it, with the possibility of rewording for clarification. Cognitive interviews are a methodology to improve survey construct validity due to their exploratory nature; they reveal reasons for respondents’ answers, and identify which questions may be being interpreted differently than investigators intend [38]. Each student received a $25 gift card as compensation for their time.

Administration

The full scale was administered to six separate cohorts of students from 2015 to 2018. Four cohorts of first-year medical students completed a confidential, online survey prior to a novel session on implicit bias introduced during orientation week. Two cohorts of third-year medical students, distinct students from the first-year cohorts, completed the same survey on paper prior to a required session on implicit bias (2016–2017). Although the third-year students were further along in their medical education, all participants took the survey prior to being exposed to any formal instruction during medical school related to IBRM. Item responses (e.g., “strongly agree”, “slightly disagree”) were assigned integer values; a student’s score was a sum of response values for all items. For each administration, a cover sheet was included explaining the voluntary nature of the study and providing information on its IRB approval.

Construct validity- internal structure

We examined evidence for item cohesion and internal structure using psychometric measures including reliability, item-total and item-rest correlations, and assessed the factor structure using exploratory factor analysis with correlated factor scores. The purpose of this analysis was to determine how well the items measured the ATIBI construct(s), and whether there was enough cohesion among items and sufficient variability in the ATIBI scores to reliably distinguish between persons with different scores. The item-total correlation is the correlation of the item’s score with the total score, which reflects how strongly the item measures the overall construct. Standard correlation is the correlation of the item with the total score if all items were standardized. The item-rest score is the item’s score correlated with the total score from the scale, minus the item’s score. This provides information about how strongly the item measures the construct while not influencing the total score. The analysis of scores and scale structure was conducted using the R Psych package [39].

Construct validity: relationship to other variables

A second convenience sample of medical student volunteers completed four additional scales in the spring of 2017. These students were a sub-set of volunteers who had taken the ATIBI and responded to an invitation to take the additional scales afterward. The first 100 students who responded were eligible. Each student received a $25 gift card as compensation for their time. To assess for convergent validity, we correlated scores between the ATIBI and four other measures. The Integrative Medicine Attitude Questionnaire (IMAQ) was selected as a convergent scale; it includes factors related to openness to new ideas, paradigms, and interactions between patients and providers [40]. It has been administered to medical students, trainees, and practicing physicians (α = 0.89) [40]. Implicit bias instruction requires learners to be open to recognizing biases about which they may have not been previously aware. In addition, in our previous work, we identified opportunities to restore patient-provider rapport should bias be perceived during an encounter, which requires an openness and attention to the dynamics of the patient-provider interaction [6]. The second convergent scale selected was the Internal Motivation to Respond Without Prejudice Scales (IMS); it measures participants personal, internal motivations regarding prejudice, and implicit bias is internally accepted and personal as well [41]. It has been administered to university students (α = 0.83) [41]. The External Motivation to Respond Without Prejudice Scale (EMS) and the Groningen Reflection Ability Scale (GRAS) were selected as a discriminant scales. EMS measures response to external pressures regarding prejudiced responses; it has been administered to university students (α = 0.78) [41]. GRAS measures self-perceived personal reflection ability, as a general skill in professional growth; it has been administered to medical students (α = 0.83) [42].

We estimated convergent and discriminant validity coefficients for the scale using a Multitrait-Multimethod matrix (MTMM), using the R base package [39]. The MTMM method for inspecting validity does not have any direct standards for interpretation. In MTMM, constructs that are expected to be similar (i.e., convergent) should have larger correlations with the construct of interest, while constructs that are expected to be dissimilar should have smaller correlations [43, 44]. In their textbook on psychometric theory, Raykov and Marcoulides [45] affirm this approach, saying that convergent validity measures should be lower than the construct’s (i.e., ATIBI) reliability, but higher than the discriminant validity coefficients: “This is consistent with theoretical expectations, given the convergent validity coefficients reflect relationships between different measures of the same trait, whereas the discriminant validity coefficients reflect considerably weaker relationships between different indicators of different constructs” [page 222]. Therefore, we expected the convergent IMAQ and IMS scores to have larger correlations with the ATIBI than the EMS and GRAS scores, which are discriminant.

A timeline of all procedures is outlined in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Timeline of all procedures for development of Attitudes Toward Implicit Bias Instrument (ATIBI) at Albert Einstein College of Medicine, New York, USA. Psychometric analysis occurred iteratively throughout

Results

Construct validity: content

Item design

Through our Delphi technique, we developed a 27-item scale. This was created from 44 items originally identified by the authors, 24 items after the first round and three additional items resulting from the second round with the expert panel. The finalized items are reported in Table 1. The expert panel determined it was imperative to include questions related to the following constructs: 1) implicit bias as a valid concept; 2) implicit bias existing within oneself; 3) the potential for implicit bias to influence clinical care; 4) the value of implicit bias in medical student education; and 5) their self-perceived confidence level in recognizing and managing one’s own implicit biases. As items were reviewed, accepted, reworded, or discarded, and additional items suggested, participants ensured that the five constructs were represented within the evolving questions.

Table 1 Items on the original administration of the Attitudes Toward Implicit Bias Instrument after expert consensus through modified Delphi-technique, 2015–2018

Item refinement

Upon completion of the ATIBI, we discussed each item with students in the initial cohort (N = 13). Three items were reworded. A second cohort of students took the ATIBI with the reworded items (N = 7). One reworded item was still confusing, and we eliminated it completely from our scale, leaving 26 items for the internal structure analysis.

Construct validity: internal structure

Sample

A total of 1281 students were eligible to participate. Students who were absent from the instructional session, could not connect via wireless internet to the survey, or for whom we had incomplete survey data were excluded, leading to a final sample of 705 first-year and 367 third-year students, for a total of n = 1072 (84% response rate). Student demographic data are presented in Table 2. There were no statistically significant differences between demographic groups (all p values ≥0.05). However, there was a significant overall difference of 3.7 points (t = 4.8(688.86), p < .001, 95% CI = [2.2, 5.2]) between first-year students (MN = 90.9, SD = 10.8) and third-year students (MN = 87.2, SD = 12.9).

Table 2 Average total scores on Attitude Toward Implicit Bias Instrument by demographic characteristics and year in medical school. Students completed the instrument between 2015 and 2018 in New York, USA

Item and reliability analysis

Although the initial reliability of the scores was high (α = 0.90), three items were removed from the scale after administration to the first cohort (N = 155 in 2015) because their scores had low correlations with the total scores, suggesting they did not measure the implicit bias construct strongly enough to keep in the questionnaire. This survey (Additional file 1) underwent further testing with a larger cohort from 2016 to 2018, another five items were removed because of low item-total correlations and no contribution to the measured factor structure. The final ATIBI contained 18 items, each scored on a scale of 1 to 6, resulting in a total possible score range of 18 to 108, with higher scores reflecting more positive attitudes. This 18-item survey was administered to the subsequent cohorts. The scores had very high reliability (α = 0.90), suggesting that responses to items across the scale are consistent. The mean score was 89.4 (out of 108) with a standard deviation of 11.7. The standard error of measurement was estimated to be 3.7, indicating a relatively small amount of measurement error. All items in general had strong correlations. The average item score and standard deviation, the item-total correlation, standard correlation and item-rest correlation (RD) are included in Table 3.

Table 3 Classical item statistics for items included in the final Attitudes Toward Implicit Bias Instrument

Factor analysis

An exploratory factor analysis of the 18-item scale (using correlated factor scores via Oblimin rotation in the R Psych package) strongly suggested five factors (based on parallel analysis results depicted in the screen plot in Fig. 2) underlie the responses to items on the ATIBI (RMSEA = 0.043, TLI = 0.97). The factor loadings and proportions of variance explained by the factors are in Table 4. The first factor, labeled Valuing Implicit Bias Instruction in Medical Education based on the pattern of item loadings, explained 14% of the total variance in the data. The second factor, Acceptance of Implicit Bias in Oneself, explained 11%, the third, Self-Awareness/Perceived Self-Efficacy related to implicit bias, explained 13%, the fourth, Recognition of the Importance of Implicit Bias, explained 12%, and the last, Acceptance of the Impact of Implicit Bias on Clinical Care, explained 4%. In total, 54% of the total variability in the data was explained by the factor structure. Table 4 also includes the communal variance, which is the proportion of each item’s variance that was accounted for by the factors. Table 5 lists the factor correlations, which are the correlations between scores on each of the factors; all factors have low-to-moderate intercorrelations, suggesting a single common higher-order factor is present.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Scree plot of factor analysis eigenvalues (vertical axis) against the factor numbers (horizontal axis). The solid line marks eigenvalues greater than 1 and the dotted line marks eigenvalues that are greater in value than resampled data that has no factor structure

Table 4 Factor loadings and variance explained from an exploratory factor analysis of Attitudes Toward Implicit Bias Instrument items
Table 5 Inter-correlations between factors within the Attitudes Toward Implicit Bias Instrument

Construct validity: relationship to other variables

The ATIBI scores were moderately convergent with the IMAQ (r = 0.63, 95% CI: [0.59, 0.66]) and IMS (r = 0.36, 95% CI: [0.32, 0.40]). This suggests that a small-but-not-insignificant proportion of the variability in the respective scores are related, suggesting that they measure some overlapping information, providing evidence for convergent validity. The ATIBI scores had lower correlations with the EMS (r = 0.15, 95% CI: [0.09, 0.19]) and GRAS (r = 0.12, 95% CI: [0.06, 0.17]), suggesting that the scores measure discriminant constructs, providing evidence for discriminant validity. Note that these correlations match the expected pattern described by Sawilowsky [44] and Raykov & Marcoulides [45], that the ATIBI reliability (α = 0.90) should be greater than the convergent correlations (0.63, 0.36), which should be in turn greater than the discriminant correlations (0.15, 0.19).

Discussion

We describe the development and provide evidence for construct validity of the Attitudes Toward Implicit Bias Instrument (ATIBI), a novel survey instrument to assess learner attitudes toward IBRM instruction. Given the efforts across health professions to address implicit bias through curricular innovation, this survey instrument is both timely and significant. We believe the ATIBI has several strengths. To our knowledge, it is the first survey assessing learner attitudes regarding IBRM instruction that provides evidence of three forms of construct validity: content, internal structure, and relationship to other variables. The Delphi expert panel consensus and the results of the cognitive interviews conducted with students provided strong evidence of content validity. The ATIBI shows evidence of construct validity related to internal structure with high reliability scores. The exploratory factor analysis suggests subscales in attitudes toward the importance of implicit bias instruction (both in general and with specific attention to recognition and management), acceptance of the presence of bias in oneself, its impact on clinical care, and recognition of systemic discrimination. Finally, the ATIBI shows evidence of construct validity for relation to other variables, being able to discriminate among similar and dissimilar constructs. The ATIBI is easy to administer electronically or on paper, and in our experience, it should take learners approximately 10 min or less to complete.

Transformative learning theory (TLT) has been proposed as an effective guide for instruction in IBRM [27]. TLT can challenge existing attitudes and facilitate a questioning of those attitudes leading to eventual paradigm shifts [46]. Briefly, TLT has four parts, an experience, critical reflection, dialogue, and behavior change [46]. The experience can be created for the learners and should be profound, a “disorienting dilemma,” in order to engage learners through the remaining phases of TLT [46, 47]. For sensitive topics such as racial bias, it is important to meet learners where they are in terms of their attitudes [48]. The ATIBI, therefore, could serve two purposes to enhance efforts in curriculum development for IBRM: 1) the ATIBI could serve as a baseline assessment of learner attitudes, thereby informing curriculum development, including the experience aspect of TLT; and 2) in aggregate it could inform program evaluations of components of curricula that target learner attitudes toward IBRM potentially identifying successful components of the curriculum and those parts requiring revision. Future uses of the ATIBI include opportunities to evaluate the instrument’s ability to assess individual learner changes over time and to use the subscales in program evaluations to evaluate specific components of curricula.

Limitations

Our study has some limitations. It is a single institution study and may not be fully generalizable across institutions (although we engaged experts in other institutions as part of our Delphi approach). Medical students at other institutions may have different attitudes. Given attitudes are self-reported, there exists the potential for social desirability bias. The item and reliability analysis are sample dependent, not all administrations of the ATIBI will return the same item and reliability statistics. Even in our own sample, the first- and third-year cohorts scored differently. We speculate that there was increasing popularity of implicit bias instruction in undergraduate education at the time, scores could be influenced by online versus paper administration, or it may be related to the differences in experience in clinical contexts between first- and third- years. Future research will endeavor to uncover the reason(s). The ATIBI was administered once for this analysis. Future efforts should evaluate its utility as a longitudinal instrument. Implicit bias is ubiquitous across health professions, and we only surveyed medical students. More research is needed to see if the instrument retains evidence for construct validity when adapted for other health professions students. Future analyses can include assessment of measurement invariance across populations (nurses, physician assistants, etc.) and confirmatory factor analysis for new cohorts to ensure the stability of psychometric coefficients over different samples. Although the items measure five factors, six or fewer items measure these constructs, which means that subscores on the five constructs measured by so few items would have very low reliability. For this reason, until further research is conducted, we are only able to suggest using the total ATIBI sum score, which is supported by the common factor in the exploratory factor analysis and the classical item statistics.

Conclusion

In conclusion, the Attitudes Toward Implicit Bias Instrument is a novel instrument that produces reliable and valid scores and may be used to measure medical students’ attitudes toward acceptance of implicit bias in oneself, implicit bias instruction, and its relevance to clinical care.

Availability of data and materials

Available from corresponding author upon request.

References

  1. Janevic T, Sripad P, Bradley E, Dimitrievska V. “There’s no kind of respect here” a qualitative study of racism and access to maternal health care among Romani women in the Balkans. Int J Equity Health. 2011;10(1):53. https://doi.org/10.1186/1475-9276-10-53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Tang SY, Browne AJ. ‘Race’ matters: racialization and egalitarian discourses involving Aboriginal people in the Canadian health care context. Ethn Health. 2008;13(2):109–27. https://doi.org/10.1080/13557850701830307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Harris R, Tobias M, Jeffreys M, Waldegrave K, Karlsen S, Nazroo J. Effects of self-reported racial discrimination and deprivation on Maori health and inequalities in New Zealand: cross-sectional study. Lancet. 2006;367(9527):2005–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(06)68890-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Mellor D, McCabe M, Ricciardelli L, Mussap A, Tyler M. Toward an understanding of the poor health status of indigenous Australian men. Qual Health Res. 2016;26(14):1949–60. https://doi.org/10.1177/1049732315609898.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Amirehsani KA, Hu J, Wallace DC, Silva ZA, Dick S, West-Livingston LN, et al. US healthcare experiences of Hispanic patients with diabetes and family members: a qualitative analysis. J Community Health Nurs. 2017;34(3):126–35. https://doi.org/10.1080/07370016.2017.1340556.

  6. Gonzalez CM, Deno ML, Kintzer E, Marantz PR, Lypson ML, McKee MD. Patient perspectives on racial and ethnic implicit bias in clinical encounters: implications for curriculum development. Patient Educ Couns. 2018;101(9):1669–75. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2018.05.016.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Maina IW, Belton TD, Ginzberg S, Singh A, Johnson TJ. A decade of studying implicit racial/ethnic bias in healthcare providers using the implicit association test. Soc Sci Med. 2018;199:219–29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.05.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Payne BK, Vuletich HA, Brown-Iannuzzi JL. Historical roots of implicit bias in slavery. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2019;116(24):11693–8. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1818816116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Project Implicit [Available from: https://implicit.harvard.edu/implicit/.

  10. Zestcott CA, Blair IV, Stone J. Examining the presence, consequences, and reduction of implicit Bias in health care: a narrative review. Group Process Intergroup Relat. 2016;19(4):528–42. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430216642029.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. van Ryn M, Hardeman R, Phelan SM, Ph DD, Dovidio JF, Herrin J, et al. Medical school experiences associated with change in implicit racial Bias among 3547 students: a medical student CHANGES study report. J Gen Intern Med. 2015;30(12):1748–56. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11606-015-3447-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Cormack D, Harris R, Stanley J, Lacey C, Jones R, Curtis E. Ethnic bias amongst medical students in Aotearoa/New Zealand: findings from the Bias and decision making in medicine (BDMM) study. PLoS One. 2018;13(8):e0201168. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0201168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Sandhu HS, Arora A, Brasch J, Streiner DL. Mental health stigma: explicit and implicit attitudes of Canadian undergraduate students, medical school students, and psychiatrists. Can J Psychiatr. 2019;64(3):209–17. https://doi.org/10.1177/0706743718792193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Boker Lund T, Brodersen J, Sandoe P. A study of anti-fat Bias among Danish general practitioners and whether this Bias and general Practitioners’ lifestyle can affect treatment of tension headache in patients with obesity. Obes Facts. 2018;11(6):501–13. https://doi.org/10.1159/000493373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Waller T, Lampman C, Lupfer-Johnson G. Assessing bias against overweight individuals among nursing and psychology students: an implicit association test. J Clin Nurs. 2012;21(23–24):3504–12. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2702.2012.04226.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Nathan ML, Ormond KE, Dial CM, Gamma A, Lunn MR. Genetic Counselors’ and genetic counseling Students’ implicit and explicit attitudes toward homosexuality. J Genet Couns. 2019;28(1):91–101. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10897-018-0295-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Teal CR, Shada RE, Gill AC, Thompson BM, Fruge E, Villarreal GB, et al. When best intentions aren't enough: helping medical students develop strategies for managing bias about patients. J Gen Intern Med. 2010;25(Suppl 2):S115–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Gonzalez CM, Kim MY, Marantz PR. Implicit bias and its relation to health disparities: a teaching program and survey of medical students. Teach Learn Med. 2014;26(1):64–71. https://doi.org/10.1080/10401334.2013.857341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Ross PT, Lypson ML. Using artistic-narrative to stimulate reflection on physician bias. Teach Learn Med. 2014;26(4):344–9. https://doi.org/10.1080/10401334.2014.945032.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Gonzalez CM, Fox AD, Marantz PR. The evolution of an elective in health disparities and advocacy: description of instructional strategies and program evaluation. Acad Med. 2015;90(12):1636–40. https://doi.org/10.1097/ACM.0000000000000850.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Leslie KF, Sawning S, Shaw MA, Martin LJ, Simpson RC, Stephens JE, et al. Changes in medical student implicit attitudes following a health equity curricular intervention. Med Teach. 2018;40(4):372–8. https://doi.org/10.1080/0142159X.2017.1403014.

  22. Zeidan AJ, Khatri UG, Aysola J, Shofer FS, Mamtani M, Scott KR, et al. Implicit Bias education and emergency medicine training: step one? Awareness AEM Educ Train. 2019;3(1):81–5. https://doi.org/10.1002/aet2.10124.

  23. Teall AM, Graham M, Jenkins N, Ali A, Pryba J, Overcash J. Faculty perceptions of engaging students in active learning to address implicit Bias using videos exemplifying the prenatal visit of a lesbian couple. J Transcult Nurs. 2019;30(6):616.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Steed R. The effects of an instructional intervention on racial attitude formation in occupational therapy students. J Transcult Nurs. 2014;25(4):403–9. https://doi.org/10.1177/1043659614523471.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Schultz PL, Baker J. Teaching strategies to increase nursing student acceptance and Management of Unconscious Bias. J Nurs Educ. 2017;56(11):692–6. https://doi.org/10.3928/01484834-20171020-11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Gonzalez CM, Deno ML, Kintzer E, Marantz PR, Lypson ML, McKee MD. A qualitative study of New York medical student views on implicit Bias instruction: implications for curriculum development. J Gen Intern Med. 2019;34(5):692–8. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11606-019-04891-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Sukhera J, Watling CJ, Gonzalez CM. Implicit Bias in health professions: from recognition to transformation. Acad Med. 2020;95(5):717–23. https://doi.org/10.1097/ACM.0000000000003173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. De Houwer J. Implicit Bias is behavior: a functional-cognitive perspective on implicit Bias. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2019;14(5):835–40. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691619855638.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Avant ND, Weed E, Connelly C, Hincapie AL, Penm J. Qualitative analysis of student Pharmacists’ reflections of Harvard's race implicit association test. Curr Pharm Teach Learn. 2018;10(5):611–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cptl.2018.02.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Sukhera J, Wodzinski M, Teunissen PW, Lingard L, Watling C. Striving While Accepting: Exploring the Relationship Between Identity and Implicit Bias Recognition and Management. Acad Med. 2018;93:S82–S8 11S Association of American Medical Colleges Learn Serve Lead: Proceedings of the 57th Annual Research in Medical Education Sessions.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Sukhera J, Milne A, Teunissen PW, Lingard L, Watling C. The actual versus idealized self: exploring responses to feedback about implicit Bias in health professionals. Acad Med. 2018;93(4):623–9. https://doi.org/10.1097/ACM.0000000000002006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Gonzalez CM, Garba RJ, Liguori A, Marantz PR, McKee MD, Lypson ML. How to Make or Break Implicit Bias Instruction: Implications for Curriculum Development. Acad Med. 2018;93:S74–81 11S Association of American Medical Colleges Learn Serve Lead: Proceedings of the 57th Annual Research in Medical Education Sessions.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Downing SM. Validity: on meaningful interpretation of assessment data. Med Educ. 2003;37(9):830–7. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2923.2003.01594.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Buck DS, Monteiro FM, Kneuper S, Rochon D, Clark DL, Melillo A, et al. Design and validation of the health Professionals’ attitudes toward the homeless inventory (HPATHI). BMC Med Educ. 2005;5(1):2. https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-6920-5-2.

  35. Blackall GF, Melnick SA, Shoop GH, George J, Lerner SM, Wilson PK, et al. Professionalism in medical education: the development and validation of a survey instrument to assess attitudes toward professionalism. Med Teach. 2007;29(2–3):e58–62. https://doi.org/10.1080/01421590601044984.

  36. Ip EH, Marshall S, Vitolins M, Crandall SJ, Davis S, Miller D, et al. Measuring medical student attitudes and beliefs regarding patients who are obese. Acad Med. 2013;88(2):282–9. https://doi.org/10.1097/ACM.0b013e31827c028d.

  37. Humphrey-Murto S, Varpio L, Gonsalves C, Wood TJ. Using consensus group methods such as Delphi and nominal group in medical education research. Med Teach. 2017;39(1):14–9. https://doi.org/10.1080/0142159X.2017.1245856.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Desimone LM, Le Floch KC. Are we asking the right questions? Using cognitive interviews to improve surveys in education research. Educ Eval Policy Anal. 2004;26(1):1–22. https://doi.org/10.3102/01623737026001001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Revelle W. Psych: procedures for personality and psychological research. 1.8.12 ed. Evanston: Northwestern University; 2018.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Schneider CD, Meek PM, Bell IR. Development and validation of IMAQ: integrative medicine attitude questionnaire. BMC Med Educ. 2003;3(1):5. https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-6920-3-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Plant EA, Devine PG. Internal and external motivation to respond without prejudice. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1998;75(3):811–32. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.75.3.811.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Aukes LC, Geertsma J, Cohen-Schotanus J, Zwierstra RP, Slaets JP. The development of a scale to measure personal reflection in medical practice and education. Med Teach. 2007;29(2–3):177–82. https://doi.org/10.1080/01421590701299272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Campbell D, Fiske DW. Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychol Bull. 1959;56(2):81–105. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0046016.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Sawilowsky SS. A quick distribution-free test for trend that contributes evidence of construct validity. Meas Eval Couns Dev. 2002;35(2):78–88. https://doi.org/10.1080/07481756.2002.12069051.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Raykov T, Marcoulides GA. Introduction to psychometric theory. New York: Taylor and Francis Group, LLC; 2011. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203841624.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  46. Mezirow J. Transformative learning: theory to practice. In transformative learning in action: insights from practice. New Directions Adult Continuing Educ. 1997;74:5–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Van Schalkwyk SC, Hafler J, Brewer TF, Maley MA, Margolis C, McNamee L, et al. Transformative learning as pedagogy for the health professions: a scoping review. Med Educ. 2019;53(6):547–58. https://doi.org/10.1111/medu.13804.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Wear D, Aultman JM. The limits of narrative: medical student resistance to confronting inequality and oppression in literature and beyond. Med Educ. 2005;39(10):1056–65. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2929.2005.02270.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank the members of the expert panel Diana Burgess, PhD, Monica Lypson, MD, MHPE, Susan Moscou, PhD, RN, Janice Sabin, PhD, LMSW, Somnath Saha, MD, MPH, Cayla Teal, PhD, and Monica Vela, MD; and the members of the Medical Education Affinity Group of the Department of Medicine, Montefiore Medical Center, for their thoughtful critique of earlier drafts of this manuscript. Dr. Jackqueline Weingarten-Arams, Ms. Donna Mahoney, and Ms. Jessica Lucas for their help in administering the surveys to students. Dr. Elisa Karp for her assistance with convergent/discriminant scales.

Previous presentations

This study was presented in oral abstract form at the annual meeting of the Society of General Internal Medicine, May 12, 2016, Hollywood, FL.

Funding

Dr. Gonzalez was supported by the Harold Amos Medical Faculty Development Program of the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation grant number AMFDP 70639, by the Macy Faculty Scholars Program of the Josiah Macy Jr. Foundation, National Institutes of Health R25HD068835–01 (Joo, P. PI) and K23MD014178 and Grants for Excellence in Medical Education, Albert Einstein College of Medicine. Dr. Marantz was supported in part by NIH/National Center for Advancing Translational Science (NCATS) Einstein-Montefiore CTSA grant numbers KL2TR001071, TL1TR001072, and UL1TR001073, and by AHRQ grant number R25HS023199.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

All authors contributed to the writing of the manuscript and reviewed the final manuscript. CMG, RJG, SB, and PRM conducted the research and JHG conducted data analysis. The author(s) read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Cristina M. Gonzalez.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All procedures performed in studies involving human subjects were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional research committee Albert Einstein College of Medicine Institutional Review Board (FWA 23382) and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. No written consent was required as this research deemed exempt research by the Albert Einstein College of Medicine Institutional Review Board (FWA 23382).

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

Authors have no competing interests to disclose.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1.

Implicit bias attitude scale.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gonzalez, C.M., Grochowalski, J.H., Garba, R.J. et al. Validity evidence for a novel instrument assessing medical student attitudes toward instruction in implicit bias recognition and management. BMC Med Educ 21, 205 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12909-021-02640-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12909-021-02640-9