Skip to main content

Self-regulated learning microanalysis for the study of the performance of clinical examinations by physiotherapy students

Abstract

Background

Students require feedback on their self-regulated learning (SRL) processes to improve the performance of clinical examinations. The key SRL processes used by students can be identified by SRL-micro-analysis but, this method has not been previously applied to physiotherapy students. The aim of this pilot study was to test a research design that might allow the evaluation of the potential usefulness of SRL microanalysis for the identification of key SRL processes used by physiotherapy students during the performance of a clinical examination skill. The objectives of the pilot study were: 1) to evaluate whether SRL-microanalysis could identify differences in the use of SRL processes between successful and unsuccessful students; 2) to evaluate the reliability of SRL microanalysis ratings produced by different assessors.

Methods

SRL-microanalysis was used with second year physiotherapy students of a Spanish university (n = 26) as they performed a goniometric task. The task required students to obtain a goniometric measurement of the shoulder joint of a peer. Two assessors evaluated student performance and conducted the SRL- microanalysis with all students. An analysis of inter-rater reliability was performed to evaluate the degree of agreement between assessors.

Results

The SRL-microanalysis revealed differences in the use of key SRL processes between successful (n = 15: 57.0%) and unsuccessful performers (n = 11: 43.0%): The differences were particularly evident in strategic planning and self-monitoring skills. There was good inter-rater reliability for scoring of strategic planning (k = 0.792), self-monitoring (k = 0.946) and self-evaluation (k = 0.846).

Conclusion

The use of SRL microanalysis characterized the key SRL processes of physiotherapy students performing a clinical skill with reliability between the assessors. This pilot study supports the potential usefulness of SRL-microanalysis for the identification of key SRL processes in physiotherapy education. Therefore, this study paves the way to the development of a full study, with a larger number of students and more diverse clinical tasks, to evaluate the SRL processes in successful and unsuccessful students.

Peer Review reports

Background

There is strong evidence across diverse contexts, from academic studies to music education and athletic training, that self–regulated learning (SRL) has an important contribution to both understanding and to inform feedback for enhancing performance [1,2,3,4].

SRL is a meta-cognitive process that has been defined as ‘self-generated thoughts, feelings, and actions that are planned and cyclically adapted to the attainment of personal goals [2]. Learners who self-regulate engage in goal-directed behaviours, use specific strategies to attain goals, and modify their goal-directed behaviours or strategies to optimise learning [2]. One of the most widely applied models of SRL was proposed by Zimmerman and is grounded in social cognitive theory [2, 5]. This model consists of 3 cyclical and iterative phases: forethought, performance, and self-reflection [6]. In the forethought phase, which takes place before the start of the task, learners anticipate the nature and complexity of the task at hand, set goals, and make specific plans to ensure appropriate performance [5]. The impetus for a learner to invest the necessary effort to engage in self-regulation is determined by self-motivation beliefs, such as self-efficacy, goal orientation, and task interest or value [1]. In the performance phase, self-regulated learners focus on monitoring and adjusting their actions. The strategies used include attention focusing, relaxation, positive self-talk, and mental rehearsal of the steps of the task [7]. In the self-reflection phase, after the task is concluded, learners self-evaluate their use of SRL processes to achieve the task and reflect on whether these processes need to be modified for enhancing future performance (7).

The use of SRL processes by learners are not amenable to direct observation but there are assessments which capture the key SRL processes that individuals employ to perform a specific task [8]. Such assessments provide useful information to enhance feedback to the learner [9]. SRL-microanalysis is designed specifically to evaluate how learners self-regulate across the three phases of the SRL cycle [10] using “think aloud protocols” during real-time observation [1, 8]. At predetermined moments, answers to questions related to the forethought, performance, and self-evaluation phases are collected and subsequently analysed [11]. SRL-microanalysis contrasts with approaches that rely solely on questionnaires, which are not designed to capture the entire SRL cycle and are subject to bias related to the beliefs of an individual in self-efficacy or attribution bias [12].

Cleary and Sandars have investigated the use of key SRL processes in medical students performing the clinical skill of venepuncture. They found that students with higher levels of strategic thinking before, during, and after the venepuncture, performed better than those with low levels of strategic thinking [13]. A narrative review of published meta-analyses of feedback interventions in education and a systematic review of effective remediation interventions in medical education have highlighted the importance of enhancing performance feedback with feedback about the use of key SRL processes by students [14, 15].

Despite the well-established importance of SRL in diverse educational contexts, including medical education, it is unknown whether poor performance of clinical skills in physiotherapy students may also be associated with difficulties in SRL. Therefore, before conducting a full study to address this gap, we developed a pilot study to evaluate the potential usefulness of SRL microanalysis in physiotherapy students. Pilot studies provide essential information about whether the rationale for a study and the proposed methods are inappropriate or overly complicated [16]. Our pilot had a focus on (a) whether our SRL-microanalysis method, can identify differences in the use of planning, monitoring and self-evaluation, between successful and unsuccessful students performing a clinical task and (b) the reliability of the SRL-microanalysis scoring made by different assessors of the students’ use of key SRL processes as they performed a clinical task.

The aim of this pilot study was to test a research design that may clarify the potential usefulness of SRL microanalysis for the identification of key SRL processes used by physiotherapy students during the performance of a clinical examination skill. The objectives of the pilot study were: 1) to evaluate whether SRL-microanalysis could identify differences in the use of SRL processes between successful and unsuccessful students; 2) to evaluate the reliability of SRL microanalysis ratings produced by different assessors.

Methods

Participants and setting

Participants were undergraduate second year physiotherapy students at the Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Las Palmas, in Gran Canaria, Spain. Students were recruited at the conclusion of a lecture by the first author (RM). The general nature of the study was explained without passing on specific information about SRL. All participants had successfully completed the “Valoración en Fisioterapia I” (Assessment in Physiotherapy 1)-UNESCO code 3211.11, within the previous 3 months, in which they had performed joint goniometric measurements similar to the clinical skill task used in this study.

The goniometric task

We chose goniometry for our study as it is a common clinical skill task. It is also well-defined within international physiotherapy curricula, for example, in the Canadian physiotherapy curriculum [17]. It consists of assessing the range of a joint’s motion by measuring the angle of motion [18]. In this study, students were instructed to obtain a goniometric measurement of the shoulder joint of a peer. This task included several actions: positioning of the peer into a correct posture, setting the goniometer in the correct position, moving the joint correctly through its range of motion, and obtaining the measurement of the range of the angle of shoulder flexion [18].

The SRL-microanalysis protocol

The SRL-microanalysis protocol followed guidelines used in medical education [7]. Before the start of the interview, the interviewer described the task to the participant. The participant would then judge their ability to perform the task on a scale from 0 to 10, and answer the strategic planning question: “Do you have any particular plans about how you will obtain the measurement?”. After answering the question, the participant would perform the task. After positioning the goniometer and prior to making any joint movement in the peer, the participant answered the self-monitoring question: “Do you think you have performed a flawless process so far or have you made any mistakes? Tell me about them”. Finally, upon task completion, two self-evaluation questions to identify self-calibration were posed. Accurate self-calibration of performance is essential to initiate any change in future performance [19]. The first question was “How satisfied are you with your current performance?” on a scale from 0 to 10 [20]. The second was an open question: “What criteria did you use to determine your satisfaction?”. Finally, students were asked to judge their performance on a scale from 0 to 10 [20] (See Table 1).

Table 1 SRL Microanalytic Assessment protocol

Data collection

This study received ethical approval from The Ethical Committee of Human Research of the ULPGC, reference CEIH-2018-01. All participants gave written informed consent before data collection began.

Prior to the observations two experienced physiotherapists (RM and DA) agreed on the expected standard of performance for the task. Independently, they marked the performance of each student as successful or unsuccessful. All answers were audio-recorded and transcribed by the first author (RM). Each SRL microanalysis session lasted from 3 to 6 min.

Data analysis

The coding scheme was established in advance following the guidelines for SRL microanalysis (for more information please see [7]). Verbal responses were recorded and subsequently coded into categories using a framework from prior research [7, 10, 11]. The responses to the open questions were coded independently [13] by two authors (RM and DA). The inter-rater agreement was calculated using kappa coefficients. Differences in coding between examiners across all SRL measures were resolved through discussion among the authors (RM, DA and MJC).

Answers to open question were coded into the following categories:

  • Strategic Planning: 1) Positioning the patient (patient focus); 2) Technical performance using the goniometer (technique focus); 3) Patient and technique combined; 4) Without a plan; 5) Do not know

  • Self-monitoring: 1) not aware of any mistakes; 2) mentions procedure related mistakes; 3) non-procedure related mistakes; 4) do not know.

  • Self-Evaluation: 1) learning originating from theoretical lectures; 2) learning originating from practical sessions; 3) learning originating from both theoretical and practical sessions; 4) Other; 5) Do not know.

To investigate the pre and post difference between students’ self-evaluation of performance (calibration), we calculated t paired sample. For the quantitative analysis, we used SPSS 21.0.

Results

Recruitment

The study enrolled 26 students, 19 were female (73.7%) and 7 were male students (26.9%). They represented 38.8% of the second-year physiotherapy class.

Task performance

There were 15 successful students (57%) and 11 (43%) unsuccessful students on the goniometric task. There were proportionally fewer female students in the unsuccessful group (n = 7) 63.6% compared to the successful group (n = 12) 80%.

SRL processes

(a) Forethought phase

In the forethought phase, most successful students [14:15 (93%)] had planned the task ahead and only one student stated no planning for performance. The plans described by the successful students fell into three categories: positioning the patient (patient focus) and correct technical performance using the goniometer (technique focus) combined (n = 6, 40%), technique focus (n = 3, 20%), or patient focus alone (n = 5, 33.3%).

We present three illustrative statements on focusing on the technique made by successful students:

017 : “I think I have a plan ... I put the goniometer first. I would ask him to raise his arm and measure it. “.

020 : “First I place the stretcher at a comfortable height, I ask the patient to get into the most comfortable position and explain what he has to do. He should be comfortable”.

015 : “Yes, I have a plan. First, I place the patient in a supine position, to be comfortable and I adjust the stretcher. Then, I put the axis of the goniometer on the lateral side of the humerus, the fixed arm parallel to the midline of the humerus... The fixed one remains there, and another moves parallel to the midline of the humerus. And I ask him for the flexion movement. And I measure it”.

In the forethought phase, six (54.5%) unsuccessful students were unable to explain their action plan or stated they had no strategy for performing the task. These students were categorised as “Without a plan”. The plans of unsuccessful students could also be categorized into technique (n = 2, 18.9%), patient (n = 1, 9.1%) or technique and patient (n = 2, 18.9%).

(b) Performance phase

The narratives of successful students were very detailed, revealing attention to the details of their performances. Successful students mentioned they were under the impression they had committed a mistake (n = 9, 60%), related to the procedure (e.g., incorrect/imperfect positioning of the goniometer (n = 6, 40%) or to their own posture or the position of the bed (non-procedural) (n = 3, 20%). There was a single successful student who did not acknowledge to have self-monitored their performance. In contrast, none of the unsuccessful students could recognize their mistakes. Answers were divided in two categories: those who explicitly mentioned they had made no mistakes (n = 5, 46%) or those who were unable to answer the question (n = 6, 54.5%). This finding suggests that the student had internalised the task to a level of expertise and the key SRL processes had become routinized. For more SRL microanalysis procedure details see Tables 2 and 3.

Table 2 Qualitative variables: Strategic planning, Self-monitoring and Self-evaluation
Table 3 Examples quotes in each phase differentiated by successful and unsuccessful students

We present two illustrative statements of self-monitoring and awareness of procedural mistakes by successful students;

06 : “I made mistakes; I think ... I have to put the goniometer in this way… I am not considering the alignment of the goniometer...”

26: “I think I am making mistakes in my posture ... maybe my leg on the stretcher.”

(c) Self- evaluation phase

There was little difference in answers by successful or unsuccessful students to the question on self-evaluation. Successful students (n = 7, 47%) were mostly focused on the importance of paying attention in lectures. An illustrative statement from a successful student:

026 : “what I remember from lectures…I should put it in the right way and if it should go in the arm or move or not...”

The median scores of successful and unsuccessful students’ self-evaluation judgments of performance (calibration) were, respectively, 6 and 8. After the task, the judgment scores were higher for successful students (median = 8) than unsuccessful students (mean = 7). The differences between the judgment of performance scores pre and post task were statistically significant (t = 2.613, p = .015) with a medium effect size (r = 0.45) [21].

There were three unsuccessful students with high judgment of performance scores before starting the task who were unable to complete the task. After the task, two of these students reduced their judgment. The other student maintained the same judgment after an unsuccessful performance. Although the student who maintained a high judgment of performance had a planned the performance, the student lacked awareness of mistakes when self-monitoring his performance. These findings suggest that the student was overconfident and poorly calibrated in his initial and final judgments in relation to his performance on the task.

The satisfaction scores were higher in successful students (mean = 8.07), than in unsuccessful students (mean = 6.27). This difference between successful and unsuccessful students was significant (t = 2.663, p = 0.014).

Inter-rater reliability

The inter-rater kappa coefficients for strategic planning (0.792), self-monitoring (0.946) and self-evaluation (0.846) were high. For internal consistency, an alpha-Cronbach coefficient of 0.846 was obtained for self-judgment prior and post task, and satisfaction post-task.

Discussion

This pilot study suggests that a full study with the same research design applying SRL microanalysis to evaluate the use of SRL by physiotherapy students as they perform a clinical skill, may uncover SRL difficulties of physiotherapy students that would otherwise be unnoticed. As expected, we found differences between unsuccessful and successful students in strategic approaches to goniometric measurements, namely in strategic planning and self-monitoring.

The differences between successful and unsuccessful students in their use of strategic planning and self-monitoring processes are in line with previous findings in medical students [10, 11, 13]. For example, in a venepuncture simulation context, Sandars and Cleary found differences in strategic approaches of Year 2 medical students [13]. The two main differences between successful and unsuccessful students were similar to our findings, with an overall difference in strategic planning and self-monitoring. The wider literature also shows that individuals who focus on their planning make better adjustments during the task, compared to those who do not plan the activity [22]. This study also relates to research in other domains like sports performance, in professional development, in musician’s performance and in medical education [3, 11, 23, 24].

Interestingly, before the performance of the task, judgment of performance was higher in unsuccessful students than successful. The literature suggests that this lack of calibration between perceived success in performing a task and their actual performance is greater in unsuccessful students than successful students [25]. The Dunning-Kruger effect, in which unsuccessful students judge their knowledge or performance as better than successful students, also applies [26]. One explanation may be that unsuccessful students think that they have all the necessary knowledge and skills, leading to premature closing of studying and practicing.

To our best knowledge, researchers have not yet applied SRL microanalysis techniques to understand students’ use of SRL processes during the performance of clinical skills in the physiotherapy context. Although we found interesting differences between the use of SRL processes between unsuccessful and successful students, our primary focus has been on methodological development reflecting the breadth of use of key SRL processes during a clinical task. First, the data suggest that SRL-microanalysis may be carried out independently by multiple assessors with high inter-rater reliability. Second, the recruitment of students was successful with around 40% of students’ participation, suggesting that scaling the number of participants should be possible. The protocol was easy and fast, taking around 8 min. The questions were short and the answers were succinct, which in turn facilitated the transcription and analysis of the data.

The incorporation of SRL-microanalysis in the diagnosis of difficulties in student performance of clinical skills could potentially enhance the effectiveness of remedial programs, by informing and directing the feedback to aspects that students need to address to enhance their performance [9, 14] . The assumption that students can develop key SRL processes is aligned with the idea that SRL interventions are one form of helping students develop as independent, lifelong learners [23].

Weakness and future research

This study shares the weaknesses of any pilot study in terms of the generalizability of findings. Our study was restricted to a small sample from one institution and, in terms of clinical skills it was restricted to goniometry of one joint. However, the consistency of our findings with previous research suggests that similar findings may also occur with studies performed on other clinical skills in physiotherapy.

This pilot study was an attempt to understand whether the use of SRL-microanalysis would add value to the identification the key SRL processes, particularly when students were unsuccessful. Our findings support the potential of applying SRL microanalysis for the characterization of the use of key SRL processes by physiotherapy students while performing a clinical skill. Important aspects of the potential usefulness of the SRL microanalysis identified by the study included [1] the identification of key SRL processes with high inter-rater reliability [2] the identification of differences in key SRL processes between successful and unsuccessful students in strategic planning and self-monitoring [3] less than 5 min of student and observer time were sufficient to obtain useful information on the use of key SRL processes. The rationale and methods used in our pilot study can inform future research, and we recommend increasing the sample size and expand to a range of different clinical skills to investigate whether our findings may be generalized and also the potential of the findings to inform feedback.

Conclusions

Our findings suggest that SRL microanalysis is a potentially useful approach to identify students’ use of key SRL processes during performance of clinical examination skills in physiotherapy. As this was a pilot study, further research with the same research design is recommended to ensure generalization as well as the reproducibility of our findings.

Availability of data and materials

All data generated or analysed during this study are included in this published article and its supplementary information are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

SRL:

Self-regulated learning

RM:

Raquel Medina

DA:

David Álamo

MJC:

Manuel João Costa

SPSS:

Statistical Package for the Social Sciences

SE:

self-efficacy

References

  1. Cleary T, Callan GL, Zimmerman B. Assesing self-regulation as a cyclical, context-specific phenomenon: Overwie and analysis of SRL microanalytic protocols. Educ Res Int. 2012:1–19.

  2. Zimmerman BJ, Cleary TJ. The role of observation and emulation in the development of athletic self-regulation. J Educ Psychol. 2000;92(4):811–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. McPherson GE, Osborne MS, Evans P, Miksza P. Applying self-regulated learning microanalysis to study musicians’ practice, Applying self-regulated learning microanalysis to study musicians’ practice. Psychol Music. 2017;26:0305735617731614.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Zimmerman BJ, Zimmerman BJ. Self-regulated learning and academic achievement: an overview. Educ Psychol. 1990;25(1):3–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Zimmerman BJ. A social cognitive view of self-regulated academic learning. J Educ Psychol. 1989;81(3):329–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Cleary TJ, Zimmerman BJ, Schunk DH. Shifting towards self-regulation microanalytic assessment: historical overview, esential features, and implications for research and practice. In: Handbook of self-regulation of learning and Performanc. UK: Abingdon; 2011. p. 329–45.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Sandars J, Cleary TJ. Self-regulation theory: applications to medical education: AMEE guide no. 58. Med Teach. 2011;33:875–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Torrano F, Gonzales M. El aprendizaje autorregulado: presente y futuro de la investigación. Rev Electrónica Investig Psico Educ. 2004;2(1):1–34.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Leggett H, Sandars J, Roberts T. Twelve tips on how to provide self-regulated learning (SRL) enhanced feedback on clinical performance. Med Teach. 2017. https://doi.org/10.1080/0142159X.2017.1407868.

  10. Cleary TJ, Durming SJ, Artino AR. Microanalytic assessment of self-regulated learning during clinical reasoning tasks: recent developments and next steps. Acad Med. 2016;91(11):1516–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Artino A, Cleary TJ, Dong T, Hemmer P, Durming S. Exploring clinical reasoning in novices: a sefl-regulated learning microanalytic assessment approach. Med Educ. 2014;48:280–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Roth A, Ogrin S, Schmitz B. Assessing self-regulated learning in higher education: a systematic literature review of self-report instruments. Educ Assess Eval Account. 2016 Aug 1;28(3):225–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Cleary T, Sandars J. Assessing self-regulatory processes during clinical skill performance: a pilot study. Med Teach. 2011;33:368–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Hattie J, Timperley H. The power of feedback. Rev Educ Res. 2007;77(1):81–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Cleland J, Leggett H, Sandars J, Costa MJ, Patel R, Moffat M. The remediation challenge: theoretical and methodological insights from a systematic review. Med Educ. 2013;47(1):242–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Thabane L, Ma J, Chu R, Cheng J, Ismaila A, Rios LP, Robson R, Thabane M, Giangregorio L, Goldsmith CH. A tutorial on pilot studies: the what, why and how. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2010 Dec 1;10(1):1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Wishart L, Harrison E, Swinamer J, MIller C. Entry-to-Practice. Physiotherapy Curriculum: Content Guidelines for Canadian University Programs [Internet]. 2009. http://www.physiotherapyeducation.ca/PhysiotherapyEducation.html. [cited 2018 Mar 16]. Available from: http://www.physiotherapyeducation.ca/Resources/National%20PT%20Curriculum%20Guidelines%202009.pdf.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Sabari JS, Maltzev I, Lubarsky D, Liszkay E, Homel P. Goniometric assessment of shoulder range of motion: comparison of testing in supine and sitting positions. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1998;79:647–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Stone NJ. Exploring the relationship between calibration and self-regulated learning. Educ Psychol Rev. 2000 Dec 1;12(4):437–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Zimmerman B, Kitsantas A. Acquiring writing revision skill: shifting from process to outcome self-rRegulatory goals. J Educ Psychol. 1999;91(2):242–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Cohen J. A power primer Jacob Cohen psychological bulletin. Psychol Bull. 1992;112(1):155–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Schunk DH. Social cognitive theory and self-regulated learning. In: Self-regulated learning and academic acvhievement. 2nd ed. New York: Springer Link; 2001. p. 125–41.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Kitsantas A, Zimmerman B. Comparing self-Reegulatory processes among novice, non-expert, and expert volleyball players: a microanalytic study. J Appl Sport Psychol. 2002;14:91–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Peters-Burton EE, Botov IS. Self-regulated learning microanalysis as a tool to inform professional development delivery in real-time. Metacogn Learn. 2017;12(1):45–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Dinsmore DL, Parkinson MM. What are confidence judgments made of? Students’ explanations for their confidence ratings and what that means for calibration. Learn Instr. 2013;24:4–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Dunning D. The Dunning-Kruger effect: On being ignorant of one’s own ignorance. In: Advances in experimental social psychology, vol. 44. San Diego: Academic Press; 2011. p. 247–96. (Advances in experimental social psychology).

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge the editorial assistance of Dr. John Yaphe of the University of Minho School of Medicine. This study was unfunded research as part of the dissertation requirements for a PhD student from University of Las Palmas of Gran Canaria.

Funding

This study was unfunded research as part of the dissertation requirements for a PhD student from University of Las Palmas. This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Concept/idea/: MC, RM. Research design: MC, R, JS. Data collection: RM, DA. Data analysis: MC, M, DA, DCF. Project management: FR, MC. Providing facilities/equipment: DA. Writing: MC, RM, DCF, JS, FR. All authors have read and approved the manuscript.

Authors’ information

Not applicable.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Raquel I. Medina-Ramírez.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study received ethical approval from The Ethical Committee of Human Research of the ULPGC, reference CEIH-2018-01. All participants gave written informed consent before data collection began.

Consent for publication

Since there were no details that would compromise anonymity, we did not request any consent for publication.

Competing interests

The authors report no conflicts of interest. The authors alone are responsible for the content and writing of the article.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Medina-Ramírez, R.I., Álamo-Arce, D.D., Rodriguez-Castro, F. et al. Self-regulated learning microanalysis for the study of the performance of clinical examinations by physiotherapy students. BMC Med Educ 20, 233 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12909-020-02149-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12909-020-02149-7

Keywords