Skip to main content

Impact of simulated patient-based communication training vs. real patient-based communication training on empathetic behaviour in undergraduate students – a prospective evaluation study

Abstract

Background

Empathy is a key competency and is essential for doctor-patient relationships. Studies have proven a continuous reduction of empathy in medical students during their study period. The use of SPs is positively evaluated for competency acquisition and real patient communication training has positive effects on empathy empowerment. Therefore, the present study focusses on the impact of simulated patient (SP) vs real patient (RP) communication training on empathetic behaviour in undergraduate medical students.

Methods

The prospective evaluation took place during a 210-minute skills lab unit on medical communication for 3rd year medical students. Study participants were allocated in advance to one of three groups: one group trained with an SP (SP-group) and was informed about the fact that it was an SP; another group trained with an SP but assumed to encounter an RP (incognito patient group (IP-group)); the last group trained with an RP and was correctly informed about it (real patient group (RP-group). Self-assessed empathy was measured by using Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy (JSPE) and Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI), as these are the most commonly used instruments for assessing empathy. Study participants were evaluated on empathetic behaviour by their group-associated patient using the Consultation and Relational Empathy (CARE) scale.

Results

146 students participated. There was no significant difference in self-assessed empathy between groups for JSPE and IRI. External assessment via CARE showed a statistically significant difference between SP-group and IP-group , as well as between SP-group and RP-group. There was no significant difference between IP-group and RP-group. This means that students training with real patients (or who believed them to be real) did receive significantly lower performance ratings on their empathy.

Conclusion

The results demonstrate a significant lower external empathy rating for students who had trained with a real patient or if they were in the belief of having encountered a real patient; this may be due to inhibitions and a lack of routine. Therefore, we recommend implementing SPs in the early study period with the gradual integration of RPs in the student’s further course of study.

Peer Review reports

Background

Studying medicine combines scientific curiosity with social and altruistic aspects. On the other hand, medical studies are traditionally oriented towards scientific content, so soft skills such as empathy and communication are scarcely emphasised and happen to be neglected in modern medicine [1,2,3,4]. In recent years, approaches and changes have already been developed to better address empathy and communication in medical teaching. These include, for example, longitudinal communication curricula, implementation guidelines and training of breaking bad news [5,6,7,8]. Nevertheless, there is still a need for improvement.

Empathy is a key competency for every doctor, regardless of the field of expertise, since the most frequent medical tasks are conversations with patients and their next of kin [9,10,11]. The empathy shown by the doctor is essential for a good doctor-patient relationship, while it also facilitates diagnostics and, therefore, has an impact on therapeutical outcomes [12, 13]. Rakel et al. showed that patients with a common cold had significantly shorter convalescence times if a high level of empathy was displayed by their treating physician; white blood cell counts (neutrophile granulocytes) and levels of interleukin 8 also decreased according to the subjective perception of the patients [14]. However, the development of empathy plays a role not only for the doctor-patient relationship but also for personal progress and mental health, since there are indications that in situations where medical student empathy is high, burnout is highly likely to be low [15].

Despite its undeniable importance, studies have proven that there is a continuous reduction of empathy in medical students during their study period [2, 3]. This is especially the case in young doctors; after starting regular patient contact, their empathy levels seem to drop. Potential reasons for this might include the high workload, high emotional stress levels and other stressors associated with entering the profession [2, 3, 16, 17].

Many countries such as Canada and the United States have already reacted to this perception by adjusting their curricula, naming empathy as an educational goal [18, 19]. In Germany, the National Competency-Based Catalogue of Learning Objectives Medicine, a nationwide standardised catalogue of learning targets, also includes empathy and communication skills, as do the sub-catalogues of certain medical fields [20]. The use of SPs has already been positively evaluated in terms of competency acquisition [21], while Ahrweiler et al. have shown that using real patients for student training and a patient-centered education also have positive effects on empathy empowerment [22].

The present study aims to compare directly the impact that simulated patient-based communication training vs. real patient-based communication training has on the empathetic behaviour in undergraduate students.

Methods

Participants and background

The present study is a prospective evaluation of the impact that simulated patient-based communication training has on empathetic behaviour in medical students.

It was performed according to the ethical principles of the World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki (Ethical Principles for Medical Research Involving Human Subjects) and was reviewed by the ethical committee of the University Hospital. As stated by the ethical committee, no further approval was required.

Study participants were undergraduate medical students in their 3rd year of studies. At this stage the majority of the students had minimum to no experience with SP training. The study took place during the mandatory surgical skills lab training to the extent of one week, which was then followed by a surgical internship of two weeks’ duration. The prerequisite for attending the skills lab and the internship was the completion of the surgical main lecture course and passing the associated written exam.

Participation in the study took place after detailed oral and written explanations and consent. Furthermore, participation in the study was voluntary and could be terminated at any time without disclosing reasons.

Communication unit

The skills lab contains 12 teaching units for basic practical medical skills, one of which is a communication training unit that instructs for correct medical history taking and the meaningful structuring of informed consent discussions for surgical interventions. This unit of 210 min duration is held by peer tutors who have been specifically trained beforehand. The quality of the units is monitored and maintained by using tutor manuals, standardised presentations and mandatory tutor training on a regular basis.

Besides the medical content of the informed consent discussions, the unit focusses on communicative competencies such as empathetic behaviour and strategies to deal with taboo-afflicted topics. It contains theoretical parts as well as practical training. First, the tutors and students work out the headings on medical history taking and informed consent discussions and discuss the required contents. Afterwards, the students practise via role-playing exercises followed by a 360° feedback on content-related aspects and social interactions with the patient. By the end of the unit, each student has taken part in at least one history taking training or has led at least one informed consent discussion. The setting of the role-playing exercise is a hospital one, either at the ward or in the emergency room, in which the students act either in the role of a young doctor or an experienced final-year student, depending on the scenario.

Empathy measurement

To evaluate empathy within a study a valid and objective tool is required. An overview on accessible tests is given by Hemmerdinger et al. [23]. For self-assessment of empathy, the Jefferson Scale of Physicians Empathy (JSPE) and the Interactivity Index (IRI) are the most common ones and these were used in the present study. The JSPE was developed especially for people with medical backgrounds, whereas the IRI is a more general measure [24, 25]. While the JSPE has its unique features, it yields significant overlap with dimensions of empathy that are relevant to patient care (perspective taking and empathic concern) of the IRI. A statistically significant correlation of a moderate magnitude exists between the total scores of the JSPE and IRI [26].

The JSPE is a standardised questionnaire with 20 items; it evaluates self-assessed empathy and the relevance of empathy for medical personnel in a medical context. This questionnaire, established by Hojat et al., is validated and especially designed for people with a medical background [24]. Questions are answered on a 7-point Likert scale, ranging from “1 = do not agree” to “7 = fully agree”. There are questions that aim for empathy as well as those that query being non-empathetic. Therefore, the latter questions have to be reversed in polarity for evaluation.

The Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) was developed by Davis et al. and contains 28 items that are rated on a 5-point Likert scale [25]. The scale ranges from “1 = strongly disagree” to “5 = highly agree”. As in the JSPE, not all questions query for being empathetic and have to be repolarised before evaluation.

In the present study, the Consultation and Relational Empathy (CARE) scale, designed by Mercer et al. especially for rating medical conversational situations by the patient, was used [27]. This is categorised as an external assessment tool for empathy rated by an involved person and contains 10 items, rated on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from “1 = fully applies” to “5 = don’t apply at all”. The question design leads to a low score if the experienced empathy was subjectively high.

Intervention

Epidemiologic data of each participant were gathered before the start of the unit. Moreover, participants were asked to fill in the JSPE and the IRI, and each generated an individual code to ensure an anonymous data collection.

Prior to the skills lab, the students were assigned to their respective groups by the Office of the Dean. The assignment took place unbiased by the principal investigator and regardless of study participation. Due to the curricular integration of the study, an individual randomisation was not possible.

According to their group allocation, the students attended the communication unit. One group undertook the role-play training with a simulated patient (SP-group) and was informed about the fact that it was an SP. Another group trained with a simulated patient but assumed that they were encountering a real patient, since both the students and the tutors had been informed so by the principal investigator (incognito patient group (IP-group)). The last group trained with a real patient and were correctly informed about this, as well as their tutors (real patient group (RP-group)). The theoretical parts and the time limits did not differ between the three groups.

The study participants were evaluated in terms of empathetic behaviour immediately after taking a medical history or an informed consent discussion with their group-associated patient using the CARE scale. All patients (SPs, IPs, RPs) were recruited from our trained SP-pool and were trained on how to use the CARE scale. When performing as SPs (SP-group) or IPs (IP-group) the patients adhered to a standardised, scripted role completely different from their personal history. As RPs (RP-group), the patients had no scripted role but reported on their own personal medical and social histories. This guaranteed that all participating assessors all had the same level of experience with the students.

They were also pre-selected according to the presence of current or past surgical conditions so that they could draw from their own experience as RPs.

All sheets were marked with the individual code of the student and gathered in a closed envelope. The evaluation only took place at the end of term to avoid recognition and linking of the sheets to the respective student.

Statistical methods

Data were processed using Microsoft Excel (Microsoft Inc., Redmond, WA, USA). Statistical analysis was performed using IBM SPSS 24 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) and measurement of the effect size was performed using G*Power (University Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany).

Tests between the groups were calculated using parametric variant analysis ANOVA.

If variant homogeneity was found, the p-values for comparing the groups were analysed using the Tukey test. In cases of variant heterogeneity, a corrected ANOVA (Welch’s t-test) was used and for p-value analyses between groups, the Games-Howell test was applied. If the data was normally distributed, the analysis of differences between the sexes and the prior training of the participants was performed by using the student’s t-test for independent samples. If not normally distributed, the data were analysed using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test for independent samples. The effect size was distributed according to Cohen’s d, based on the means and standard deviations.

Results

Epidemiologic data

A total of 189 students attended the skills lab during the study period and 146 gave consent for study participation; of these, 38.4% were males, the median age being 22.9 ± 2.8 years. The study population was, therefore, representative of a 3rd year undergraduate semester at Frankfurt Goethe University Hospital. While gathering the epidemiologic data, participants were asked to provide information about their pre-educational experience. To simplify the data, these were clustered into fields: medical and/or social field (e.g. nurse, paramedic), other pre-educational experience, and none. There were no significant differences within the three study cohorts.

Results of empathy measurement

Self-assessed empathy

Table 1 demonstrates the results of the self-assessment via the JSPE and IRI subscales. The higher the score, the higher was the empathetic self-assessment of the student. There were no statistically significant differences between the groups within the items of the subscales.

Table 1 Self-assessment via JSPE and IRI

External assessment of empathy

Table 2 displays the assessment results of the students by the group-associated patient using the CARE scale. The maximum score is 5, and the minimum 1. In this case, the lower the score, the more empathetic the student was rated by the group-associated patient.

Table 2 External assessment using the CARE-Scale

The results show a significant difference between SP-group and IP-group (p < 0.001; Cohen’s d: 0.99), as well as between SP-group and RP-group (p = 0.001; Cohen’s d: 0.71), both of which have a high effect size. There was no significant difference between IP-group and RP-group (p = 0.45; Cohen’s d: 0.25). Accordingly, students who trained with real patients (or who believed the patients to be real) were rated as less emphathetic (i.e. they had a significantly higher score on the CARE scale). [Figure 1 near here]

Fig. 1
figure 1

External assessment using the CARE-Scale (SP = simulated-patient-group; IP = incognito-patient-group; RP= real-patient-group). Y-axis displays CARE-Scale rating. Data are presented as mean + standard deviation

Discussion

Our results demonstrate a significantly lower external empathy rating for students if they had trained with a real patient or were in the belief of having encountered a real patient when training. This might seem surprising at first when taking into account former published results on this topic. Ahrweiler et al. showed that using real patients for student training and a patient-centred education have positive effects on empathy empowerment [22]. Nevertheless, many studies prove a significant decrement of empathy over the medical training period [2, 3, 16, 17]. Hojat et al. described the beginning of bedside training as the time with the highest decrement in empathy [28]. This also seems to be a crucial aspect of the present study. The skills lab training is placed at the beginning of the clinical medical training, therefore, the communication unit is in many cases the first real patient encounter for the students. The lack of routine and, as a result, the accompanying stress may have contributed to inhibitions on the student’s side while working with a real patient, thus, leading to the lower ratings.

It is already known that higher stress levels can lead to lower personal performance. A review by LeBlanc et al. concluded that stress is a main impairment factor in terms of memory, multi-tasking and decision making, although how a person handles stress is a highly individual process [29].

The use of SPs as well as RPs has already been positively evaluated in terms of competency acquisition [21, 22]. Based on the present study, it seems important to plan consciously the occupation of SPs and RPs within the curriculum blueprint regarding the development of communication skills, especially empathy training. Using SPs in the early study period could create a safe space to attune the students to patient encounters and to lower their initial existing inhibitions. Subsequently, as the students’ knowledge develops and having acquired advanced skills, RPs should be gradually integrated into their training. However, further studies are needed to define in more detail the transition period from SP to RP.

The implementation of (simulated) patient’s feedback to foster empathetic behaviour development seems to be beneficial. The SP’s feedback to students has been found to be positively evaluated and appreciated, [30, 31] thus, some faculties already rely on the assessment of SPs [32]. These positive effects might facilitate the gain of confidence and development of empathy in the early phases of the students’ training.

Another reason for lower empathy rates in the RP-group could arise from the personal rating tendencies of the assessor. Assessments by real patients can be stricter due to a higher personal involvement and, therefore, hold higher expectations than the assessment of an SP. This phenomena is known as the hawk-dove effect and describes different rating tendencies in different persons that are mostly due to biographical differences [33]. One can assume that a real patient, talking about their own, often emotionally afflicted history, demands higher requirements from the student’s empathy than an SP who is not emotionally involved. Under this assumption, however, one would also have expected a difference between IP- and RP-group. There are various possible explanations for this. One might be that the students possibly demonstrate a higher level of empathy in known testing scenarios with the SP but with a real or perceived real patient, thus correspondingly less practice artificiality, they begin to forget they are being tested and start to show a more intuitive behavior. Fuller et al. showed in an interview based study that one factor that influences empathy competence is the hierarchical position, since communicators often “communicate from their place in the hierachy“ [34]. The subjective perception of this position may shift for a student between an SP and an RP, which might contribute to a change in empathy.

On the side of the assessors, the specificity of the selected RPs, who were also all trained SPs, may play a role. Simmenroth-Nayda et al. conducted an interview study on the effects of being an SP on real patient life [35]. They found them to be more attentive, having a better understanding of the circumstances under which doctors work and to act more self-confidently. This insider knowledge may have led to a weakening of the biographical hawk-dove effect and thus milder assessments as a real patient. Further studies and replications in the future may help to better assess this aspect.

A noteworthy limitation of this study is the narrow focus, determined by just one type of skills training unit within one semester period. However, the study includes the results of nearly a whole cohort of medical students at the faculty without selectional bias, and, therefore, is still eligible for making valid deductions.

There are numerous studies that analyse the impact that student’s personality traits and emotion recognition abilities have on their demonstrated empathy [36]. Another interesting aspect would be an evaluation of the patient’s own personality traits and empathy levels and their impact on the assessment. This aspect was not covered in this study and should, therefore, be subject to further research.

Conclusion

Our results demonstrate a significant, lower external empathy rating for students if they had trained with a real patient, or were in the belief of having encountered a real patient when in training; this low empathy rating was possibly due to their inhibitions and lack of routine. Based on these data, the thesis can be put forward that it could be beneficial to impelent SPs in the early study period with the gradual integration of RPs in the further course of communication training and empathy development. However, further exploration of the observed mechanisms is needed.

Data availability

Data is provided within the manuscript. The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

CARE:

Consultation and Relational Empathy

IRI:

Interpersonal Reactivity Index

JSPE:

JEFFERSON Scale of Physician Empathy

RP:

Real patient

SP:

Simulated patient

References

  1. Reynolds WJ, Scott B. Do nurses and other professional helpers normally display much empathy? J Adv Nurs. 2000;31:226–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Neumann M, Edelhauser F, Tauschel D, et al. Empathy decline and its reasons: a systematic review of studies with medical students and residents. Acad Med. 2011;86:996–1009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Hojat M, Shannon SC, DeSantis J, Speicher MR, Bragan L, Calabrese LH. Does Empathy decline in the clinical phase of Medical Education? A Nationwide, Multi-Institutional, cross-sectional study of students at DO-Granting medical schools. Acad Med. 2020;95:911–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Iorio S, Cilione M, Martini M, Tofani M, Gazzaniga V. Soft skills are hard Skills-A historical perspective. Med (Kaunas) 2022;58.

  5. Bundesministerium für Gesundheit. Kommunikative Kompetenzen von Ärztinnen und Ärzten - Leitfaden zur Implementierung des nationalen longitudinalen Mustercurriculums Kommunikation in der Medizin. https://www.bundesgesundheitsministerium.de/fileadmin/Dateien/5_Publikationen/Gesundheit/Broschueren/Leitfaden_Kommunikative_Kompetenzen_med._Ausbildung.pdf3(2020) Accessed 15 July 2024.

  6. Sator M, Junger J. [From stand-alone solution to longitudinal communication curriculum–development and implementation at the Faculty of Medicine in Heidelberg]. Psychother Psychosom Med Psychol. 2015;65:191–8.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Dale MacLaine T, Lowe N, Dale J. The use of simulation in medical student education on the topic of breaking bad news: a systematic review. Patient Educ Couns. 2021;104:2670–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Howick J, Slavin D, Carr S, et al. Towards an empathic hidden curriculum in medical school: a roadmap. J Eval Clin Pract. 2024;30:525–32. https://doi.org/10.1111/jep.13966.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Kappauf HW. Kommunikation in Der Onkologie [Communication in oncology]. Hautarzt. 2004;55:709–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Steinhausen S, Ommen O, Thum S, et al. Physician empathy and subjective evaluation of medical treatment outcome in trauma surgery patients. Patient Educ Couns. 2014;95:53–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Street RL. Jr. How clinician-patient communication contributes to health improvement: modeling pathways from talk to outcome. Patient Educ Couns. 2013;92:286–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Beckman HB, Frankel RM. Training practitioners to communicate effectively in cancer care: it is the relationship that counts. Patient Educ Couns. 2003;50:85–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Coulehan JL, Platt FW, Egener B, et al. Let me see if i have this right… words that help build empathy. Ann Intern Med. 2001;135:221–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Rakel D, Barrett B, Zhang Z, et al. Perception of empathy in the therapeutic encounter: effects on the common cold. Patient Educ Couns. 2011;85:390–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Cairns P, Isham AE, Zachariae R. The association between empathy and burnout in medical students: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Med Educ. 2024;24:640.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Hojat M, Mangione S, Nasca TJ, et al. An empirical study of decline in empathy in medical school. Med Educ. 2004;38:934–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Ian Jeffrey D. Exploring empathy with medical students. 1st ed. London, UK: Palgrave Macmillan; 2019.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  18. Association of American Medical Colleges. Learning objectives for medical student education - guidelines for medical schools. https://store.aamc.org/downloadable/download/sample/sample_id/144 (1998). Accessed 3 Jul 2023.

  19. Frank JR, Danoff D. The CanMEDS initiative: implementing an outcomes-based framework of physician competencies. Med Teach. 2007;29:642–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Kadmon M, Bender M, Adili F, et al. Nationaler Kompetenzbasierter Lernzielkatalog Chirurgie (NKLC)–Eine Chance Zur Verbesserung Der Studentischen Ausbildung [National competence based learning goals catalogue surgery - an opportunity to improve student education]. Zentralbl Chir. 2016;141:355–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Kaplonyi J, Bowles KA, Nestel D, et al. Understanding the impact of simulated patients on health care learners’ communication skills: a systematic review. Med Educ. 2017;51:1209–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Ahrweiler F, Neumann M, Goldblatt H, Hahn EG, Scheffer C. Determinants of physician empathy during medical education: hypothetical conclusions from an exploratory qualitative survey of practicing physicians. BMC Med Educ. 2014;14:122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Hemmerdinger JM, Stoddart SD, Lilford RJ. A systematic review of tests of empathy in medicine. BMC Med Educ. 2007;7:24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Hojat M, Mangione S, Nasca TJ, et al. The Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy: development and preliminary psychometric data. Educ Psychol Meas. 2001;61:349–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Davis MH. Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a multidimensional approach. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1983;44:113–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Hojat M, Mangione S, Kane GC, Gonnella JS. Relationships between scores of the Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy (JSPE) and the interpersonal reactivity index (IRI). Med Teach. 2005;27:625–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Mercer SW, McConnachie A, Maxwell M, Heaney D, Watt GC. Relevance and practical use of the Consultation and Relational Empathy (CARE) measure in general practice. Fam Pract. 2005;22:328–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Hojat M, Vergare MJ, Maxwell K, et al. The devil is in the third year: a longitudinal study of erosion of empathy in medical school. Acad Med. 2009;84:1182–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. LeBlanc VR. The effects of Acute stress on performance: implications for Health professions Education. Acad Med. 2009;84:S25–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Wright B, McKendree J, Morgan L, Allgar VL, Brown A. Examiner and simulated patient ratings of empathy in medical student final year clinical examination: are they useful? BMC Med Educ. 2014;14:199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Bokken L, Rethans JJ, van Heurn L, Duvivier R, Scherpbier A, van der Vleuten C. Students’ views on the use of real patients and simulated patients in undergraduate medical education. Acad Med. 2009;84:958–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Ladyshewsky R. Simulated patients and assessment. Med Teach. 1999;21:266–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Chong L, Taylor S, Haywood M, Adelstein BA, Shulruf B. The sights and insights of examiners in objective structured clinical examinations. J Educ Eval Health Prof. 2017;14.

  34. Fuller M, Kamans E, Van Vuuren M, Wolfenberger M, De Jong MDT. Conceptualizing empathy competence: a professional communication perspective. J Bus Tech Commun. 2021;35:333–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Simmenroth-Nayda A, Marx G, Lorkowski T, Himmel W. Working as simulated patient has effects on real patient life - preliminary insights from a qualitative study. GMS J Med Educ.2016;17.

  36. Guilera T, Batalla I, Forné C, Soler-González J. Empathy and big five personality model in medical students and its relationship to gender and specialty preference: a cross-sectional study. BMC Med Educ. 2019;19:1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the participating students and SPs in this study.

Funding

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

VB: Conceptualization, Writing- Original draft preparation, Writing – Review & Editing; JS: Conceptualization, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – Review & Editing; YK: Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing – Review & Editing, Conceptualization; TS: Validation, Formal analysis, Data curation, Writing – Review & Editing. MCS: Project administration, Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – Review & Editing; UZ: Investigation, Resources, Writing- Review and Editing; RDV: Conceptualization, Writing - Review & Editing. KS: Conceptualization, Writing - Review & Editing.; MR: Conceptualization, Methodology, Supervision, Formal analysis, Ressources, Writing - Review & Editing.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Vanessa Britz.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The present study was performed according to the ethical principles of the World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki (Ethical Principles for Medical Research Involving Human Subjects) and was reviewed by the ethical committee of the University Hospital. As stated by the ethical committee, no further approval was required. Participation in the study took place after detailed oral and written explanations and consent. Furthermore, participation in the study was voluntary and could be terminated at any time without disclosing reasons.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Britz, V., Sterz, J., Koch, Y. et al. Impact of simulated patient-based communication training vs. real patient-based communication training on empathetic behaviour in undergraduate students – a prospective evaluation study. BMC Med Educ 24, 870 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12909-024-05801-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12909-024-05801-8

Keywords