Skip to main content

Insights into diagnostic errors in endocrinology: a prospective, case-based, international study

Abstract

Background

Diagnostic errors in internal medicine are common. While cognitive errors have previously been identified to be the most common contributor to errors, very little is known about errors in specific fields of internal medicine such as endocrinology. This prospective, multicenter study focused on better understanding the causes of diagnostic errors made by general practitioners and internal specialists in the area of endocrinology.

Methods

From August 2019 until January 2020, 24 physicians completed five endocrine cases on an online platform that simulated the diagnostic process. After each case, the participants had to state and explain why they chose their assumed diagnosis. The data gathering process as well as the participants’ explanations were quantitatively and qualitatively analyzed to determine the causes of the errors. The diagnostic processes in correctly and incorrectly solved cases were compared.

Results

Seven different causes of diagnostic error were identified, the most frequent being misidentification (mistaking one diagnosis with a related one or with more frequent and similar diseases) in 23% of the cases. Other causes were faulty context generation (21%) and premature closure (17%). The diagnostic confidence did not differ between correctly and incorrectly solved cases (median 8 out of 10, p = 0.24). However, in incorrectly solved cases, physicians spent less time on the technical findings (such as lab results, imaging) (median 250 s versus 199 s, p < 0.049).

Conclusions

The causes for errors in endocrine case scenarios are similar to the causes in other fields of internal medicine. Spending more time on technical findings might prevent misdiagnoses in everyday clinical practice.

Peer Review reports

Background

Diagnosing patients is a key competence of physicians. Establishing a correct diagnosis is the basis to select the best treatment for the patient. Nonetheless, diagnostic errors in medicine are frequent [1] and can have serious consequences for patients and their health [2]. An estimate from the National Academy of Medicine stated that most people will experience at least one diagnostic error in their lifetime, sometimes with severe consequences [3]. Therefore, more efforts into understanding the nature of diagnostic errors are crucial in order to reduce their occurrence and develop effective interventions. There is consensus amongst researchers that diagnostic errors are caused by both system and cognitive factors [2, 4]. Cognitive factors are considered the most common factor [2, 4,5,6]. According to Graber et al. they account for 74% of errors [4] and mainly occurred due to faulty synthesis, faulty data gathering, and faulty knowledge [4, 7]. Furthermore, errors often occur in the patient-physician encounter, including history taking and physical examination [6]. There is not yet consensus regarding the type of cognitive errors causing misdiagnosis. While some studies have suggested that cognitive biases (short cuts in the reasoning process) are the most common [4], others suggest that a lack of knowledge is the more important underlying factor [8,9,10]. Most of those studies involved a retrospective analysis of real clinical cases, which are sensitive to hindsight bias and may impact the physician´s critical assessment [11].

Clinical Reasoning – the ability to solve clinical cases – is not a general problem-solving skill [12] but it is case-specific. Therefore, it can be assumed that the kind of clinical encounters chosen for a study will influence the frequency and nature of diagnostic errors. One widely accepted theory explaining the cognitive processes in clinical reasoning is the dual processing theory [13]. Cognitive processes are controlled by two systems: System I, which is intuitive, fast and automatic, and System II, which is analytical and logical. Depending on the clinical experience and the familiarity with a specific clinical case, a physician will primarily use system I (for routine cases) or system II (for more unusual cases).

The endocrine field contains some common diseases, affecting millions of people each year, such as endocrine hypertension, diabetes mellitus or osteoporosis, but also very rare diseases (such as Cushing’s syndrome or pheochromocytoma), some of them potentially fatal, if misdiagnosed [14]. While content specific endocrine knowledge is often important for a correct diagnosis, patients with endocrine diseases often first present in a general practice or in general internal medicine. It is unclear how precisely physicians in general internal medicine or general practice can diagnose endocrine more umcommon cases and whether they are able to correctly identify “red flags”. We, therefore, chose to focus on endocrine cases in this study. Specifically, we developed a mix of cases that included diseases that are known to be commonly underdiagnosed although they are quite frequent, such as primary aldosteronism [15, 16] and hyponatremia, and potentially life-threatening, rare diagnoses that require quick diagnosis (Cushing’s syndrome, pheochromocytoma and Addison’s disease).

In order to study the reasoning process of general practitioners and general internists, we conducted a study in which participants prospectively solved endocrine cases in a virtual setting. The aim was to analyze the cognitive causes of diagnostic errors in for the participants unusual cases and to identify differences between correctly and incorrectly solved cases in the field of endocrinology.

Materials and methods

Design and participants

From August 2019 until January 2020, 24 physicians practicing internal medicine or general medicine completed a total of 111 simulated online clinical cases. The cases were all endocrine, however the participants were unaware of this, they were only informed that they were internal medicine cases.

The participants were chosen amidst one specific criterion, they had to be a physician practicing internal medicine, this included all subspecialties of internal medicine, as well as general medicine, excluding solely those practicing endocrinology, as it was expected that endocrinologists would make less errors, due to their better knowledge of endocrinological diseases and the goal of this study was to analyze as many diagnostic errors as possible. Apart from that all physicians practicing internal and or general medicine were included, regardless of their level of working experience, age or origin. The participants were recruited mainly through the listserv of the SIDM (society to improve diagnosing in medicine), as well as through flyers in the LMU (Ludwig MaximiIians University) hospital, as well as through directly contacting physicians from university clinics or general practitioner practices, using contact details provided on the respective websites. The participations did not receive a financial incentive.

Case development

The cases were all written by one author (JF) based on real patient cases. Three resident and attending physicians specialized in endocrinology reviewed the cases. In an initial pilot study there were ten cases, completed by four physicians practicing internal medicine. The responses of the pilot were excluded from the data analysis. The aim of the pilot was to test the cases and the feasibility of the study. The five cases where most errors were made in the pilot, hence the most difficult, were selected for the actual study. Two cases with frequent diseases and three cases with very rare diseases were chosen for the study (Table 1). All cases are shown in the supplement.

Table 1 Overview of cases and contents of patient file

Study procedure

Participants first completed a sociodemographic questionnaire. Subsequently they diagnosed the five simulated internal medicine (endocrine) clinical cases (Table 1) on the online based platform CASUS [23]. This platform (details are shown in the supplement Fig. 1) enables the following of different steps of the diagnostic process. Each clinical case consisted of a patient history, a detailed physical examination and technical findings, i.e. results from laboratory and imaging, in the patient file (Table 1 for contents). The information in the history taking and physical examination consisted of age, gender, body mass index (BMI), vital parameters (blood pressure, heart rate, respiratory rate, body temperature) pre-existing illnesses, history of alcohol and nicotine consumption, cardiovascular, abdominal, lung and lymph node examination, a neurological examination, and their general and nutritional state. They were instructed to only look at the technical findings they deemed useful or essential to finding the correct diagnosis, in order to simulate the limited resources such as technical examinations and financial means in medical practice. However, the number of technical examinations that could be seen was not restricted. The number of technical findings viewed was recorded on the platform. Participants then had to state a diagnosis for each case, including an explanation and also had to indicate their diagnostic confidence (on a scale of 1–10, where 1 was not confident at all and 10 was very confident). They were able to switch between cases as they wished and were instructed to spend – very roughly - about 30 min on all cases to simulate the scarcity of time in medical practice, hence the time expenditure per case was measured. However, the time on task was not restricted.

Content and statistical analysis

The content and the diagnostic steps were analyzed as described in detail in a previous study regarding diagnostic errors made by students [24]. The CASUS platform allows for gathering data prospectively and then analyze the physician’s diagnostic skills and diagnostic process. In addition, the technical findings participants looked at and how much time they spent on each finding was monitored. This, along with the explanation as to why physicians chose a diagnosis, helped understand in which part of the diagnostic process errors occurred.

The causes of diagnostic errors were ascribed to one error cause based on an already published classification, which was developed as an adaption of Graber’s diagnostic errors classification [24]. The seven error categories are: inadequate knowledge base, inadequate diagnostic skills, faulty context generation, overestimating/underestimating, faulty triggering, misidentification and premature closure. For a detailed description on the development of these error categories see Braun et al. [24]. More details on how the errors were assigned to a category can be found in the supplement (supplement Table 1).

Diagnostic explanations were qualitatively analyzed [25]. Each diagnostic error was assigned to one category (Table 2). We assigned each case only to one category by choosing the predominant error that finally caused the misdiagnosis. One investigator (JF) coded all errors. A second rater (LB) also independently coded all errors and explanations. The interrater coefficient analyzed with Cohens Kappa was 0.79. The causes of misdiagnoses were quantitatively assessed.

Table 2 Frequency of causes for diagnostic errors

Diagnoses were binary coded as correct or incorrect. Cases with correct and incorrect diagnosis were compared regarding the time spent on a case, number of technical findings viewed, and diagnostic confidence.

Means and standard deviations were calculated to describe continuous variables. Absolute counts and percentage shares were applied for describing categorical variables. P-values of equal or less than 5% were considered significant. Statistical analysis was performed in SPSS 27. Differences between groups were tested by the Mann-Whitney-U-Test due to a lack of normal distribution.

Results

Participants

24 (18 male, 6 female) participants completed 111 cases in total, 9 cases were not completed. Their mean age was 45 years (SD ± 15.6). Most participants were general practitioners (36%) or working in general internal medicine (21%) whereas the remaining participants were specialized in other fields of internal medicine (Table 3). Their working settings included both hospitals and practices.

Table 3 Sociodemographic overview of participants (n = 24)

Results of the error analysis: frequency, nature and distribution of errors

The physicians misdiagnosed 52 out of 111 times, with a total error frequency of 47%. The mean time expenditure per case was 9 min and 48% of the technical findings were viewed. The frequencies of different causes for errors is shown in Table 2. Overall, the most common error type in all completed cases were misidentification and faulty context generation. Amongst the five cases, case 3 (Addison’s disease) had the lowest error frequency (33%) and case 2 (ectopic Cushing’s) had the highest (73%). The leading cause of diagnostic error differed from case to case (Table 4). In cases with rare diseases, a lack of knowledge was not more frequently a cause of errors compared to the cases with more frequent diseases.

Table 4 Kind of incorrect diagnoses

Misdiagnoses in different diseases

The kind of misdiagnoses were evaluated to explore possible patterns (Table 4). In all five cases, misdiagnoses were mostly very common diagnoses. For example, primary hyperaldosteronism was most often diagnosed as primary arterial hypertension. The misidentification with other rare diseases (pheochromocytoma) was less common. In the case of ectopic Cushing’s syndrome, physicians made the most misdiagnoses, mostly due to the fact, that they did not diagnose accurately enough: A lot of physicians diagnosed a Cushing’s syndrome, but did not classify it as an ectopic Cushing’s syndrome (but rather as Cushing’s syndrome, Cushing’s disease or pituitary tumor). When determining the error types for this case, various participants overlooked the lung mass in the thoracic X-ray, which was essential to finding the correct diagnosis. In the case SIADH, other common diagnoses were stated instead of the correct one (pregnancy, gastroenteritis). In the pheochromocytoma case, one of the clinical signs – tachycardia and atrial fibrillation – was stated as final diagnosis, although it was a symptom of the underlying disease.

Correctly and incorrectly solved cases

The time per case, the time spent on patient’s history or physical examination did not differ between correctly and incorrectly solved cases (Table 5). Furthermore, the number of technical findings that were looked at by the physicians did not differ between the cases, but in correctly solved cases, physicians spent more time on these technical findings.

Table 5 Comparison of correctly and incorrectly solved cases

The diagnostic confidence was very high both in the correctly and incorrectly solved cases (median diagnostic confidence: 8 out of 10).

Discussion and conclusion

Causes of errors in endocrinology

We were able to distinguish seven different cognitive error types. Overall, the most common error categories were misidentification, premature closure and faulty context generation. These findings are in line with previous studies [2, 4]:

In this study, all misdiagnoses were assigned only to one category, so that a single root cause of the nature of the error was determined. It is a strength of the study that the reflections of the physicians are available. This is an insight that most studies, particularly retrospective ones, do not have. By analyzing the explanations, we could determine the cause of an error with more certainty.

However, in other studies, it was also described that errors are often multifactorial [4, 26]. Hence it could be that certain error causes are interdependent. For instance, if physicians have a cognitive bias due to overconfidence, such as stereotyping based on certain information of the patient, this could lead to premature closure, where they do not look closely at further technical findings, as they have already come to a premature diagnosis, hence leading to over- and underestimating of certain information. Other studies suggest lack of diagnostic skills (e.g. interpretation of imaging) may even be an underlying factor of premature closure [14].

Not all diagnostic errors in endocrinology are of the same severity. For example, in the case of a patient with ectopic Cushing’s syndrome, the most errors occurred. However, the most common misdiagnosis – Cushing’s syndrome - is not completely incorrect but just imprecise. In everyday clinical practice, it is important for general practitioners and physicians working in general internal medicine to identify the correct specialty from which the disease could originate, so that the patient can be transferred to a specialist. An endocrinologist will be able (in the majority of cases) to classify this patient correctly as a patient having ectopic Cushing’s syndrome. In clinical practice, this incorrectness will possibly not harm the patient. However, in other cases with rare diseases such as pheochromocytoma the misdiagnoses might have much severer consequences. This should be kept in mind when we analyze diagnostic errors: Not every error will harm a patient. Therefore, just the frequency of errors is not critical but the causes of the errors, the kind of misdiagnoses and the therapy decisions based on those errors. A wrong diagnosis can nevertheless result in a correct treatment as already shown [27].

Correctly and incorrectly solved cases

Interestingly, we observed very little differences regarding time on task or number of technical findings viewed between correctly and incorrectly solved cases, which suggests that there are no major differences in the reasoning process of correct and incorrect diagnosis, but it likely depends more on the knowledge of physicians. It is notable that in correctly solved cases, physicians spent more time on technical examinations such as laboratory results and imaging. Spending more time on these findings might prevent premature closure and faulty context generation. This finding is different from several previous studies where correct diagnoses were often based on a faster diagnostic process [28, 29]. One explanation could be that in this study the cases were endocrine and therefore outside the main clinical expertise of participants. They may have led to a closer review of the technical findings in cases where physicians were correct. An interesting finding is that the levels of confidence in the diagnosis were rather high in both correct and incorrect diagnosis. The fact that physicians have poor calibration between confidence and accuracy is in line with previous studies [30, 31]. However, the overall confidence levels seem very high, reflecting a strong overconfidence of physicians in this study. Also, a low learning motivation can be associated with overconfidence as recently shown [32], which should be kept in minding in previous studies.

Strengths and limitations

Strengths of this study are the prospective design and the comprehensive analysis of the causes of errors enabled by the study platform CASUS. Moreover, as many of the participants were general practitioners, who patients often initially consult, this study simulates the primary care situation. Also, we had a multicentric, international approach, which is another advantage of the study.

The limitations of the study include the limited sample size, as a sample of 24 physicians may not accurately represent the broader population of healthcare professionals. It has to be considered that our sample size may not accurately represent the broader population of healthcare professionals. The selection of the participants might be influenced by availability or interest. However, as quite a large number of cases was analyzed and we were able to draw valid conclusions of that which might be addressed in upcoming larger studies.

However, we qualitatively analyzed 111 cases, which is quite extensive. Furthermore, in this study, the CASUS platform, although realistic, does present an artificial setting. Therefore, we cannot be sure that the findings apply to clinical practice. It is a constraint of the study, whether the findings can truly be seen to represent cognitive processes in genuine clinical encounters. For example, participants could not profit from discussions with colleagues, which could help find the correct diagnosis [33]. Additionally, in everyday clinical practice, errors can be multifactorial and more unpredictable as more variables influence the outcome. However, even in everyday practice there are errors only caused by cognitive factors (see Graber et al.) and at least for those instances, the results of the study seem applicable. Additionally, we focused on difficult endocrinological cases, therefore, our results regarding the causes of errors might not be applicable for more common and less difficult endocrinological encounters. However, the cases chosen for a study will always influence the results regarding diagnostic errors as clinical reasoning is indeed case-specific. Therefore, this is a general limitation not only affecting our study. A further limitation is that we may not capture the full spectrum of endocrinological cases encountered in clinical practice. Also, there is the possibility of certain participants being familiar with the casus interface, and hence allowing them to more easily navigate through the findings, however the instructions on how to navigate through the findings are easily comprehendible.

Outlook and conclusion

To the best of our knowledge, errors in endocrinology made by physicians were analyzed for the first time in a controlled setting in this prospective study. Predominant causes for errors in this specialty include misidentification, faulty context generation, premature closure and a lack of knowledge which is in line with previous findings on causes of diagnostic error. The process of correctly diagnosed cases did not differ much from the incorrectly diagnosed cases. This suggests that it is not the reasoning process that is different in cases with and without errors, but that it is the content specific knowledge that plays the most important role. Therefore, practice with a large variety of clinical cases (including endocrine cases) in continuing medical education seems advantageous for error reduction.

Data Availability

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

  1. Graber ML, Carlson B. Diagnostic error: the hidden epidemic. Physician Exec. 2011;37(6):12–4.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Zwaan L, de Bruijne M, Wagner C, Thijs A, Smits M, van der Wal G, et al. Patient record review of the incidence, consequences, and causes of diagnostic adverse events. Arch Intern Med. 2010;170(12):1015–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Medicine, Io. National Academies of Sciences E, Medicine. In: Balogh EP, Miller BT, Ball JR, editors. Improving diagnosis in health care. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2015:472.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Graber ML, Franklin N, Gordon R. Diagnostic error in internal medicine. Arch Intern Med. 2005;165(13):1493–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Zwaan L, Singh H. The challenges in defining and measuring diagnostic error. Diagnosis (Berlin Germany). 2015;2(2):97–103.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Singh H, Giardina TD, Meyer AN, Forjuoh SN, Reis MD, Thomas EJ. Types and origins of diagnostic errors in primary care settings. JAMA Intern Med. 2013;173(6):418–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Kuhn J, van den Berg P, Mamede S, Zwaan L, Bindels P, van Gog T. Improving medical residents’ self-assessment of their diagnostic accuracy: does feedback help? Advances in health sciences education: theory and practice. 2022;27(1):189–200.

  8. Zwaan L, Thijs A, Wagner C, van der Wal G, Timmermans DR. Relating faults in diagnostic reasoning with diagnostic errors and patient harm. Acad Medicine: J Association Am Med Colleges. 2012;87(2):149–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Mamede S, de Carvalho-Filho MA, de Faria RMD, Franci D, Nunes M, Ribeiro LMC, et al. Immunising’ physicians against availability bias in diagnostic reasoning: a randomised controlled experiment. BMJ Qual Saf. 2020;29(7):550–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Mamede S, Goeijenbier M, Schuit SCE, de Carvalho Filho MA, Staal J, Zwaan L, et al. Specific disease knowledge as predictor of susceptibility to availability bias in diagnostic reasoning: a randomized controlled experiment. J Gen Intern Med. 2021;36(3):640–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Zwaan L, Monteiro S, Sherbino J, Ilgen J, Howey B, Norman G. Is bias in the eye of the beholder? A vignette study to assess recognition of cognitive biases in clinical case workups. BMJ Qual Saf. 2017;26(2):104–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. ten Cate O, Durning SJ. Understanding clinical reasoning from multiple perspectives: a conceptual and theoretical overview. In: ten Cate O, Custers E, Durning SJ, editors. Principles and practice of case-based clinical reasoning education: a method for preclinical students. Cham (CH): Springer Copyright 2018, The Author(s). 2018:35–46.

  13. Norman G. Dual processing and diagnostic errors. Advances in health sciences education: theory and practice. 2009;14 Suppl 1:37–49.

  14. Bajwa SJ, Jindal R. Endocrine emergencies in critically ill patients: challenges in diagnosis and management. Indian J Endocrinol Metabol. 2012;16(5):722–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Handgriff L, Reincke M. [Primary aldosteronism - why is it still underdiagnosed in clinical practice?]. Deutsche medizinische Wochenschrift (1946). 2020;145(11):716 – 21.

  16. Buffolo F, Monticone S, Tetti M, Mulatero P. Primary aldosteronism in the primary care setting. Current opinion in endocrinology, diabetes, and obesity. 2018;25(3):155–9.

  17. Käyser SC, Dekkers T, Groenewoud HJ, van der Wilt GJ, Carel Bakx J, van der Wel MC, et al. Study heterogeneity and estimation of prevalence of primary aldosteronism: a systematic review and meta-regression analysis. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2016;101(7):2826–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Steffensen C, Bak AM, Rubeck KZ, Jorgensen JO. Epidemiology of Cushing’s syndrome. Neuroendocrinology. 2010;92(Suppl 1):1–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Newell-Price J, Bertagna X, Grossman AB, Nieman LK. Cushing’s syndrome. Lancet (London England). 2006;367(9522):1605–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Willis AC, Vince FP. The prevalence of Addison’s disease in coventry, UK. Postgrad Med J. 1997;73(859):286–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Pinkhasov A, Xiong G, Bourgeois JA, Heinrich TW, Huang H, Coriolan S, et al. Management of SIADH-related hyponatremia due to psychotropic medications - an expert consensus from the association of medicine and psychiatry. J Psychosom Res. 2021;151:110654.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Chen H, Sippel RS, O’Dorisio MS, Vinik AI, Lloyd RV, Pacak K. The North American neuroendocrine tumor society consensus guideline for the diagnosis and management of neuroendocrine tumors: pheochromocytoma, paraganglioma, and medullary thyroid cancer. Pancreas. 2010;39(6):775–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Fischer MR, Aulinger B, Baehring T. Computer-based-training (CBT): Fallorientiertes Lernen am PC mit dem CASUS/ProMediWeb-System. DMW-Deutsche Medizinische Wochenschrift. 1999;124(46):1401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Braun LT, Zwaan L, Kiesewetter J, Fischer MR, Schmidmaier R. Diagnostic errors by medical students: results of a prospective qualitative study. BMC Med Educ. 2017;17(1):191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Mayring P, editor. Editor combination and integration of qualitative and quantitative analysis. Forum qualitative sozialforschung/forum: qualitative social research; 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Hautz WE, Kämmer JE, Hautz SC, Sauter TC, Zwaan L, Exadaktylos AK, et al. Diagnostic error increases mortality and length of hospital stay in patients presenting through the emergency room. Scand J Trauma Resusc Emerg Med. 2019;27(1):54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Tausendfreund O, Braun LT, Schmidmaier R. Types of therapeutic errors in the management of osteoporosis made by physicians and medical students. BMC Med Educ. 2022;22(1):323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Staal J, Alsma J, Mamede S, Olson APJ, Prins-van Gilst G, Geerlings SE, et al. The relationship between time to diagnose and diagnostic accuracy among internal medicine residents: a randomized experiment. BMC Med Educ. 2021;21(1):227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Sherbino J, Dore KL, Wood TJ, Young ME, Gaissmaier W, Kreuger S, et al. The relationship between response time and diagnostic accuracy. Acad Medicine: J Association Am Med Colleges. 2012;87(6):785–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Meyer AN, Payne VL, Meeks DW, Rao R, Singh H. Physicians’ diagnostic accuracy, confidence, and resource requests: a vignette study. JAMA Intern Med. 2013;173(21):1952–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Staal J, Speelman M, Brand R, Alsma J, Zwaan L. Does a suggested diagnosis in a general practitioners’ referral question impact diagnostic reasoning: an experimental study. BMC Med Educ. 2022;22(1):256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Trifunovic-Koenig M, Bushuven S, Gerber B, Otto B, Dettenkofer M, Salm F et al. Correlation between overconfidence and learning motivation in postgraduate infection prevention and control training. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2022;19(9).

  33. Radkowitsch A, Fischer MR, Schmidmaier R, Fischer F. Learning to diagnose collaboratively: validating a simulation for medical students. GMS J Med Educ. 2020;37(5):Doc51.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

J.F. prepared the study material, collected, prepared and analyzed the data, she also wrote the initial version of the manuscript. L.B. designed the study, analyzed the data, conducted the statistics and revised the initial version of the manuscript. L.H. prepared the study material. B.K. conducted the statistics. M.F, M.R. and L.Z. revised the manuscript. L.Z. also helped in the conceptualization of the study design and the recruitment of participants. R.S. designed the study, prepared the study material and revised the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Leah T. Braun.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The local Institutional Review Board of the Ludwig-Maximilians-University deemed the study exempt from approval. Informed consent was obtained from all individuals included in this study. All methods were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations in the Declaration of Helsinki.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary Material 1: Supplement Figure 1:

The CASUS surface. Supplement Table 1: The different error categories with explanation. Supplement: Cases Case 1: Morbus Conn. Case 2: Ectopic Cushing’s syndrome(paraneoplastic due to small cell lung cancer). Case 3: Morbus Addison. Case 4: SIADH (caused by medication, due to citalopram). Case 5: Pheochromocytoma

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Frey, J., Braun, L.T., Handgriff, L. et al. Insights into diagnostic errors in endocrinology: a prospective, case-based, international study. BMC Med Educ 23, 934 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12909-023-04927-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12909-023-04927-5

Keywords