Skip to main content

The undue influence of genetic information on senior medical students’ treatment decisions

Abstract

Background

Knowledge of the genetic basis of health conditions can influence how the public perceives their own and others’ health. When there are known genetic associations for such conditions, genetic essentialist biases facilitate deterministic thinking and an over-emphasis of genetic causality. This study investigates the role that genetic essentialist biases play in medical decision-making.

Methods

Senior postgraduate medical students (N = 102) read a scenario in which a patient presents with gastroenterological symptoms. Half of the students were told that the patient tested positive for HLADQ2 – a gene implicated in, but not deterministic of, coeliac disease. The other half received no genetic information. Students were assessed on their recommendations for investigation and management using a multiple-choice questionnaire. Twenty-two of these students participated in a qualitative follow-up which used focus groups and semi-structured interviews to explore the reasoning behind students’ responses.

Results

Management recommendations differed between the two groups, with those receiving genetic information more likely to recommend a gluten free diet. Recommendations for further investigation did not differ significantly between groups. Interviews suggested that these findings arose despite the students’ good understanding of the common non-deterministic nature of genes, such as HLADQ2.

Conclusion

Differences in management recommendations suggest that the inclusion of genetic information unduly biased students towards a premature diagnosis of a serious health condition, coeliac disease. Follow-up interviews introduced the possibility that observed manipulation-based differences may have been based on anticipated expectations of examiners, rather than perceived future clinical practice. Based on the present results it is unclear whether intentional exam-taking strategies fully account for medical students’ decisions, or if they contribute in addition to the activation of genetic essentialist biases. Further research in clinical settings may ascertain whether genetic essentialist biases would truly influence medical student and doctors within their clinical practice environment.

Peer Review reports

Background

Diagnostic errors are common across all medical disciplines, resulting in potential and actual patient harms, increased health system costs, and reduction of patient trust [18]. Cognitive biases can significantly affect diagnostic reasoning and can result in diagnostic error [7, 17]. Such biases in clinical practice can occur as a result of the manner in which information is presented [33], systemic prejudices directed towards a social or cultural group (e.g. [9]), or the practitioner’s mental state [30]. At the time of writing the Centre of Evidence Based Medicine has indexed over 60 different kinds of biases in their ‘Catalogue of Bias’ [8]. Many of these have not yet been explored in a clinical context.

Recent work in lay populations illustrates the effect that genetic information has on people’s beliefs, decisions, affect, and behaviours. These effects can be explicated through the Genetic Essentialist Framework (GEF). Myriad of phenomena (e.g., traits, health conditions) perceived to have genetic associations are viewed as more predetermined. When both genetic and environmental causes can be identified, individuals often tend to prioritise the causal role of genes, and perceive those traits as more immutable and inevitable [12, 19]. These biases can influence the perception of self and others [15, 23, 25], behaviour [2, 10, 28], and cognition [11]. For instance, individuals who received feedback about their own purported genetic predispositions to depression reported reduced feelings of control [6], less confidence in their ability to cope [27], and remembered more depressive symptoms in their recent past [1, 26]. Simlar patterns were found with people’s responses to learning about purported genetic predisposition to alcoholism [14].

Genetic information influences the perceived efficacy of medical treatments from the patient perspective. When medical conditions are thought to have a genetic component, non-biogenic treatments, such as psychotherapy, diet or exercise are often discounted [2, 22]. This, in combination with the fact that biased decision making can also be elicited via diagnostic suggestion from patients [16], suggests that genetic essentialist biases could impact clinical decision making in a world in which patients have increased access to their personal genetic information.

To understand how genetic essentialist biases might play out in a clinical setting, this study examined the influence of genetic information on the clinical decision-making of final-year medical students. The medical students were given genetic information in the form of medical vignettes around a patient presenting with gastrological symptoms. These symptoms could have been caused by coeliac disease, but were likely to be due to more common diseases.

To better understand how genetic information influences students’ clinical decision making, we further explored their educational experience using semi-structured interviews to understand the reasoning behind students’ responses. This included how expectations and experiences of medical school and life as a practitioner influenced their responses in the initial experiment.

Both phases were designed to illuminate whether the presentation of genetic information influenced students’ diagnostic and treatment decisions, and to understand the decision making processes underlying those decisions in an examination context.

Methods

Participants and recruitment

In phase 1, 482 senior medical students from the Sydney Medical Program were offered the opportunity to take part in a 60-min revision session on gastroenterology. Students were in Stage 3 (Year 3 and Year 4) of a four-year (full-time) postgraduate Doctor of Medicine (MD) degree. The students were two weeks from undergoing their summative barrier examinations, ensuring that their knowledge levels were optimal prior. A sample of 102 students were randomly allocated to read one of two hypothetical cases (henceforth the scenarios) in Phase 1 of the study. 22 of these students were followed up for Phase 2 of the study.

Phase 1

Under examination conditions in the form of an online learning module, participants read one of two almost identical realistic scenarios describing a patient with a 3-month history of lethargy, occasional diarrhoea, and confabulated feelings. The sole difference between the scenarios was that a HLADQ2 positive result from an at-home genetic test was included in one scenario. HLADQ2 is a necessary but not sufficient criterion for the development of coeliac disease. Whereas 30–40% of European-descent persons carry the HLADQ2 (or HLADQ8) allele, only around 1% have coeliac disease [21].

Following this, participants completed ten multiple-choice questions, including two specific questions regarding 1) what the appropriate follow-up investigation would be; and 2) what the appropriate management would be (Appendix 1, questions 7 and 9 respectively). Following the completion of the questions, participants were fully debriefed about the module being a part of a research endeavour to investigate the effect of added genetic information.

To explore whether providing information about HLADQ2 influenced treatment decisions, a chi square test assessed the ratio of the students who (erroneously) opted to recommend “gluten free diet” as the most appropriate management of the patient symptoms, as a function of the inclusion of genetic information (indicating that they have likely prematurely diagnosed the patient with coeliac disease).

To ascertain whether the differences observed above were a function of overall better understanding of the medical case among the group of students who were randomly assigned to not receive the HLADQ2 information, a t-test was performed on the aggregated scores of the eight questions that should not have been affected by that information.

Phase 2

Phase 2 facilitated a qualitative phenomenological exploration of participants’ educational experience, focussing on their responses to the relevant multiple-choice questions, using a mixture of face-to-face focus-groups or personal interviews (based on students’ preference and availability). The sample size (N = 22) was derived using thematic saturation considerations [31].

Each focus-group or interview was conducted 2–3 weeks after the initial study, giving participants the opportunity to reflect on their experiences. Semi-structured interviews were conducted, audio recorded, and transcribed. Data was analysed using Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis (IPA) [36], a data analysis method designed to assess, among other, how educational sense-making translates into clinical practice [24]. IPA aims to understand the participant’s lived experience during the educational module to provide insight that may not be captured using alternative research methods. In IPA studies, small homogenous samples of participants are used and systematically analysed to identify patterns of convergence and divergence [36]. The results are presented in the form of a narrative account where the researcher’s interpretation is supported by quotations. The analytical process in IPA is an iterative and inductive cycle that starts by examining the particular and being descriptive and then ascends towards examining the shared and being interpretive. The following is a brief description of the process: Close line by line reading of transcripts, becoming familiar with their content, data reduction through descriptive and conceptual coding of the text, identification of emergent patterns and relationships between codes in light of the research question, code reduction and recombination into emergent sub-themes, development of a structure illustrating the relationships between themes.

Ethics application was approved by the University of Sydney HREC, protocol number 2019/239, and informed consent was obtained from all subjects prior to participation.

Results

Phase 1

102 students participated in phase 1 of the study. 60 were male, 52 female; 17 in their third-year and 85 in their fourth-year of postgraduate study. Participants ranged in age from 23 to 43 (M = 26.39 years; SD = 3.09).

62.0% of the students who were not told about HLADQ2 opted to recommend the diet, whereas 84.6% of the students provided with the HLADQ2 information did, χ2(1) = 6.70, p = 0.01. The students provided with the HLADQ2 information (63%) were also more likely to erroneously recommend coeliac-related treatments than those who were not (50%), but this difference did not reach significance, χ2(1) = 1.88, p = 0.17.

The mean number of correct answers among the students who were given the HLADQ2 information (M = 3.77, SE = 0.21) did not significantly differ from those who did not see this information (M = 3.54, SE = 0.23), t(100) = 0.75, p = 0.46.

Phase 2

In phase 2, 22 students were divided into four focus-groups of four students each, and six individual interviews. Analysis of the data demonstrated two superordinate themes: ‘Preparation for future practice’ and ‘My genetic understanding’. Each superordinate theme was made up of two separate themes. The superordinate themes and themes are shown in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Superordinate themes and themes

Superordinate theme 1: Preparation for future practice

This superordinate theme represents the students’ experiences of their medical training, which seems to have taught them to behave differently in the classroom and in clinical practice. It was made up of two separate themes: 1) the technique of passing the exam, which described the students aiming to identify examiners’ expectations rather than offer a more appropriate clinical recommendation, and 2) becoming a holistic clinician, which described the students’ recognition of their apprenticeship career stage. Tables 1 and 2 show a selection of supportive quotes for the two themes described above.

Table 1 Supportive quotes for theme 1 ‘the technique of passing the exam’
Table 2 Supportive quotes for theme 2 ‘becoming a holistic clinician’

Superordinate theme 2: My genetic understanding.

This superordinate theme represents the students’ understanding of the complexity of genetic aetiology and its associated role in health and disease. It was made up of two separate themes 1) genetic disease outcomes, which described the students’ recognition of the influence of genetic markers on disease, and 2) genetic information usage, which described the students’ hesitancy to use genetic information as a diagnostic tool. Tables 3 and 4 show a selection of supportive quotes for the themes.

Table 3 Supportive quotes for theme 1 ‘genetic disease outcomes’
Table 4 Supportive quotes for theme 2 ‘genetic information usage’

Discussion

As predicted, medical students exposed to genetic information were significantly more likely to recommend management strategies guided by inappropriate genetic inferences. Taken in isolation, this result suggests that students viewed HLADQ2 more deterministically than they should have, downplaying other possible causal agents. Such a response, predicted by two genetic essentialist biases – determinism and specific aetiology – are in line with Dar-Nimrod and Heine’s [12] genetic essentialist framework. It extends the literature on genetic essentialist biases from the focus on lay populations [13] to medical professionals performing clinical reasoning. As initial evidence for genetic essentialist biases playing a role in medical decision making, it raises concerns about potential serious diagnostic and management errors. Instances of cognitive bias leading to diagnostic error are not confined to the deterministic misinterpretation of genetic information. For instance, the misinterpretation of ANA positivity in the diagnosis of SLE is well recognised, since ANA positivity can be considered necessary (as ANA negative SLE is extremely rare) but insufficient for the diagnosis [29, 34].

However, contrary to expectations, there was no significant difference between the two groups in their recommendations for follow up investigation. The majority of students across both groups responded (incorrectly) in-line with an investigation of coeliac disease, including those who received no genetic information in the patient vignette. This result may indicate a primary limitation to the testing question and/or the education the students received in reference to these kinds of clinical presentations. The directionality of the findings was in line with the hypothesis, so it may have been that insufficient statistical power obscured actual empirical support for the prediction.

The results from Phase 1 are consistent with recent studies which suggest that medical students possess insufficient knowledge of genetics, especially around clinically orientated concepts such as genetic testing and genetic counselling [3]. As such, they support the suggestion for greater integration of genetics into the clinical years of medical school curricula, in line with a recent topic review from Wolyniak and colleagues, which concluded that there is a need for a development of scientific critical thinking skills, that allow students to apply foundational genetic knowledge and ethical principles to patient encounters [38].

However, our qualitative analysis reveals a potential alternative account. The superordinate themes demonstrate that students have a raw, yet nuanced understanding of gene-action in the medical context. They acknowledge the non-deterministic influence of genes such as HLADQ2, and the importance of other causal forces for disease aetiology. The supportive quotes for superordinate theme 1, ‘the technique of passing the exam’ indicate that the reason for their incorrect answers was not due to their lack of knowledge or reflective of their future clinical practice. Instead they indicated that they felt they needed to answer in line with the (perceived) examiners’ expectations, rather than the way they would act in clinical practice. The supportive quotes for superordinate theme 2 ‘being a holistic clinician’ outline how students expressed frustration with MCQs being insufficient to demonstrate their understanding of a topic in the clinical environment, which is a common criticism of this assessment modality [4].

This suggests that genetic essentialist biases may not be driving the results in Phase 1 of the study, despite them confirming the initial hypothesis. However, while the students demonstrated an awareness of non-deterministic gene-phenotype relationships in Phase 2 (superordinate theme 2 – my genetic understanding), this does not exclude the possibility that genetic essentialist biases played some role in their initial decision making.

It is possible that the explicitly reasoned justifications of conscious performative exam performance were post-hoc justifications that do not accurately reflect the reasoning processes that occurred at the time of testing, as decades of research in both psychology [5, 20] and neuroscience [35] suggest that we often offer misinformed explanations for actions we have taken when we do not recognise relevant aspects that affected our actions. Those deeper reflections may also arise once the students were informed about the true purpose of the study, and by extension, to their demonstration of a cognitive bias.

Another interesting finding from the qualitative analysis is that students perceive patients presenting with genetic information to be biased and fallible in their own understanding of that information. More research is required to understand how this perception might impact on their own clinical decision making. While diagnostic suggestions can bias medical decision making [16], it is not known whether suggestions that are perceived as biased, such as a patient’s genetic understanding, have the same effect.

Both perceived expectations (of assessors and patients) reflect a different aspect of genetic essentialist biases – the perception that others are engaging with genetic information in a biased manner. It has long been recognised that the provision of truthful and accurate information is necessary for patients to achieve sufficient health literacy beyond the merely functional level [37]. Erroneous interpretations of either patient-directed and acquired or practitioner-initiated genetic information may lead to flawed and deterministic decision-making, and become a barrier to achieving a truly interactive critical appraisal of one’s personal characteristics – a new ‘structural barrier to health’ [32]. Hence, the current findings have significant implications for both medical education and medical practice and warrant further research.

Conclusion

As expected, the presentation of genetic information influenced the clinical decision making of medical students, leading to them being more likely to recommend management strategies guided by inappropriate genetic inferences. This has significant ramifications for medical education and assessment, since there is an increasing amount of genetic content within the biomedical aspects of medical curricula, and an ongoing push to increase this content into the clinical learning aspects of medical programs.

However our study also showed that the students had a raw, yet nuanced understanding of gene-action in the medical context. Students communicated that a conscious exam-taking strategy, motivated by the belief that the needed to answer in line with perceived examiner’s expectations, explained erroneous results when presented with genetic information.

It is not possible from this study to determine if conscious exam-taking strategy fully accounts for these results, or if post-hoc reflective interpretation and rationalisation of their cognitive decision-making occurred after genetic essentialist biases influenced exam performance.

Curiously, students indicated a perception of others having genetic essentialist biases – including patients and the perceived examiner. The perception that the examiner had expectations in line with an essentialistic interpretation of genetics points to an unconsidered indirect influence of genetic essentialist thinking on clinical decision making. Further research disentangling the influences of direct (where the students are subject to essentialistic biased thinking about genetics themselves) and indirect (where students are perceiving others to think essentialisitically) is a fruitful area to research which warrants further investigation.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated and/or analysed during the current study are not publicly available due to potential re-identfiable transcripts, but some date is available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

  1. Ahn W, Bitran A, Lebowitz M. Effects of genetic information on memory for severity of depressive symptoms. PLoS ONE. 2020;15(10):e0239714. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239714.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Ahn W, Lebowitz MS. An experiment assessing effects of personalized feedback about genetic susceptibility to obesity on attitudes towards diet and exercise. Appetite. 2018;120:23–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2017.08.021.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Alotaibi AA, Cordero MAW. Assessing Medical Students’ Knowledge of Genetics: Basis for Improving Genetics Curriculum for Future Clinical Practice. Adv Med Educ Pract. 2021;12:1521–31. https://doi.org/10.2147/AMEP.S337756.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Anderson J. For Multiple Choice Questions. Med Teacher. 1979;1(1):37–42. https://doi.org/10.3109/01421597909010580.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Banaji MR, Hardin C, Rothman AJ. Implicit stereotyping in person judgment. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1993;65(2):272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Birchwood M, Mason R, MacMillan F, Healy J. Depression, demoralization and control over psychotic illness: A comparison of depressed and non-depressed patients with a chronic psychosis. Psychol Med. 1993;23(2):387–95. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291700028488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Bornstein BH, Emler AC. Rationality in medical decision making: A review of the literature on doctors’ decision-making biases. J Eval Clin Pract. 2001;7(2):97–107. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2753.2001.00284.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Catalogue of Bias. (2017, March 27). Catalogue of Bias. https://catalogofbias.org/

  9. Chiaramonte GR, Friend R. Medical students’ and residents’ gender bias in the diagnosis, treatment, and interpretation of coronary heart disease symptoms. Health Psychol. 2006;25(3):255–66. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.25.3.255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Dar-Nimrod I, Cheung BY, Ruby MB, Heine SJ. Can merely learning about obesity genes affect eating behavior? Appetite. 2014;81:269–76. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2014.06.109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Dar-Nimrod I, Heine SJ. Exposure to Scientific Theories Affects Women’s Math Performance. Science. 2006;314(5798):435–435. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1131100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Dar-Nimrod I, Heine SJ. Genetic essentialism: On the deceptive determinism of DNA. Psychol Bull. 2011;137(5):800–18. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021860.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Dar-Nimrod I, Kuntzman R, MacNevin G, Lynch K, Woods M, Morandini J. Genetic essentialism: The mediating role of essentialist biases on the relationship between genetic knowledge and the interpretations of genetic information. Eur J Med Genet. 2021;64(1):104119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejmg.2020.104119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Dar-Nimrod I, Zuckerman M, Duberstein PR. The effects of learning about one’s own genetic susceptibility to alcoholism: a randomized experiment. Genet Med. 2013;15(2):132–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Dietrich S, Matschinger H, Angermeyer MC. The Relationship between Biogenetic Causal Explanations and Social Distance toward People with Mental Disorders: Results from a Population Survey in Germany. Int J Soc Psychiatry. 2006;52(2):166–74. https://doi.org/10.1177/0020764006061246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Durning SJ, Artino AR, Boulet JR, Dorrance K, van der Vleuten C, Schuwirth L. The impact of selected contextual factors on experts’ clinical reasoning performance (does context impact clinical reasoning performance in experts?). Adv Health Sci Educ. 2012;17(1):65–79. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10459-011-9294-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Gopal DP, Chetty U, O’Donnell P, Gajria C, Blackadder-Weinstein J. Implicit bias in healthcare: Clinical practice, research and decision making. Future Healthcare Journal. 2021;8(1):40–8. https://doi.org/10.7861/fhj.2020-0233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Graber ML, Franklin N, Gordon R. Diagnostic error in internal medicine. Arch Intern Med. 2005;165(13):1493–9. https://doi.org/10.1001/archinte.165.13.1493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Heine SJ, Dar-Nimrod I, Cheung BY, Proulx T. Chapter Three - Essentially Biased: Why People Are Fatalistic About Genes. In JM Olson (Ed). Adv Experimental Soc Psychol. 2017;2017(55):137–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.aesp.2016.10.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Higgins ET, Rholes WS, Jones CR. Category accessibility and impression formation. J Exp Soc Psychol. 1977;13(2):141–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Husby S, Koletzko S, Korponay-Szabó IR, Mearin ML, Phillips A, Shamir R, Troncone R, Giersiepen K, Branski D, Catassi C, Lelgeman M, Mäki M, Ribes-Koninckx C, Ventura A, Zimmer KP. European Society for Pediatric Gastroenterology, Hepatology, and Nutrition Guidelines for the Diagnosis of Coeliac Disease. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr. 2012;54(1):136–60. https://doi.org/10.1097/MPG.0b013e31821a23d0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Iselin M-G, Addis ME. Effects of Etiology on Perceived Helpfulness of Treatments for Depression. Cogn Ther Res. 2003;27(2):205–22. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023513310243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Kvaale EP, Gottdiener WH, Haslam N. Biogenetic explanations and stigma: A meta-analytic review of associations among laypeople. Soc Sci Med. 2013;96:95–103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.07.017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Lane AS, Roberts C. Phenomenological study of medical interns reflecting on their experiences, of open disclosure communication after medication error: Linking rationalisation to the conscious competency matrix. BMJ Open. 2020;10(5):e035647. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2019-035647.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Lebowitz MS, Ahn W. Effects of biological explanations for mental disorders on clinicians’ empathy. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2014;111(50):17786–90. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1414058111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Lebowitz MS, Ahn W. Testing positive for a genetic predisposition to depression magnifies retrospective memory for depressive symptoms. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2017;85(11):1052–63. https://doi.org/10.1037/ccp0000254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Lebowitz MS, Ahn W. Blue Genes? Understanding and Mitigating Negative Consequences of Personalized Information about Genetic Risk for Depression. J Genet Couns. 2018;27(1):204–16. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10897-017-0140-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Lebowitz MS, Appelbaum PS, Dixon LB, Girgis RR, Wall MM. Experimentally exploring the potential behavioral effects of personalized genetic information about marijuana and schizophrenia risk. J Psychiatr Res. 2021;140:316–22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.05.066.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Lesuis N, den Broeder A, van Vollenhoven R, Vriezekolk J, Hulscher M. Choosing wisely in daily practice: A mixed methods study on determinants of antinuclear antibody testing by rheumatologists. Scand J Rheumatol. 2017;46(3):241–6. https://doi.org/10.1080/03009742.2016.1190983.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Loewenstein G. Projection Bias in Medical Decision Making. Med Decis Making. 2005;25(1):96–105. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989X04273799.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Moser A, Korstjens I. Series: Practical guidance to qualitative research. Part 3: Sampling, data collection and analysis. Eur J Gen Pract. 2018;24(1):9–18. https://doi.org/10.1080/13814788.2017.1375091.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Nutbeam D. Health literacy as a public health goal: A challenge for contemporary health education and communication strategies into the 21st century. Health Promot Int. 2000;15(3):259–67. https://doi.org/10.1093/heapro/15.3.259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. O’Sullivan ED, Schofield S. Cognitive Bias in Clinical Medicine. J R Coll Physicians Edinb. 2018;48(3):225–31. https://doi.org/10.4997/JRCPE.2018.306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Rajendran R, Salazar JH, Seymour RL, Laposata M, Zahner CJ. Overutilization and underutilization of autoantibody tests in patients with suspected autoimmune disorders. Diagnosis. 2021;8(4):497–503. https://doi.org/10.1515/dx-2020-0139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Searleman A. A review of right hemisphere linguistic capabilities. Psychol Bull. 1977;84(3):503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Smith JA, Flowers P, Larkin M. Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis: Theory, Method and Research. SAGE. 2009.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Wittink H, Oosterhaven J. Patient education and health literacy. Musculoskeletal Science and Practice. 2018;38:120–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.msksp.2018.06.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Wolyniak MJ, Bemis LT, Prunuske AJ. Improving medical students’ knowledge of genetic disease: A review of current and emerging pedagogical practices. Adv Med Educ Pract. 2015;6:597–608. https://doi.org/10.2147/AMEP.S73644.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to acknowledge DAATS for their transcript services.

Funding

This project was made possible through the support of a grant from the John Templeton Foundation. The views expressed in this publication are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the views of the John Templeton Foundation.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Andrew S. Lane – design of study, led the data collection and qualitative analysis, manuscript writing. Kate E. Lynch – design of study and manuscript writing. Mark Arnold—design of study and manuscript writing. Ilan Dar-Nimrod—design of study, statistical analysis, and manuscript writing. James Morandini—design of study and manuscript writing. Stefan A. Gawronski—design of study and manuscript writing. Paul E. Griffiths—design of study and manuscript writing. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Andrew Stuart Lane.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Ethics application was approved by the University of Sydney HREC, protocol number 2019/239, informed consent was obtained from all subjects prior to participation.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lane, A.S., Lynch, K.E., Arnold, M. et al. The undue influence of genetic information on senior medical students’ treatment decisions. BMC Med Educ 23, 938 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12909-023-04895-w

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12909-023-04895-w

Keywords