Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Need for cognitive closure, tolerance for ambiguity, and perfectionism in medical school applicants

Abstract

Background

Physicians have to deal with uncertainty on a daily basis, which requires high tolerance for ambiguity. When medical decisions have to be made in ambiguous situations, low levels of need for cognitive closure and high levels of adaptive perfectionism are beneficial. It might be useful to measure such personality traits during medical school selection processes. In our study, we explored the expression of need for cognitive closure, tolerance for ambiguity, and perfectionism in medical school applicants who participated in a multiple mini-interview selection process with respect to the final decision of admission or rejection.

Methods

After participating in the multiple mini-interview procedure (HAM-Int) at Hamburg Medical School in August 2019, 189 medical school applicants filled out a questionnaire including the Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale by Hewitt and Flett (MPS-H), the Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale by Frost (MPS-F), the Tolerance for Ambiguity Scale (TAS), the 16-Need for Cognitive Closure Scale (16-NCCS), and sociodemographic data. After the final admission decision, the scores of need for cognitive closure, tolerance for ambiguity, and perfectionism of admitted and rejected applicants were compared. We also assessed the predictive power of need for cognitive closure and age for the admission decision in a binary logistic regression model.

Results

Compared to the admitted applicants, the rejected applicants showed a significantly higher need for cognitive closure (p = .009). A high need for cognitive closure correlated significantly positively with maladaptive perfectionism (p < .001) and significantly negatively with tolerance for ambiguity (p < .001). Low need for cognitive closure and older age were associated with a positive admission decision.

Conclusions

Regarding the personality traits need for cognitive closure, tolerance for ambiguity, and perfectionism we identified interesting differences and correlations of relevance for physicians’ daily work in medical school applicants who were admitted or rejected after participating in a multiple mini-interview selection procedure. Further studies are needed to investigate these characteristics and their development longitudinally in medical students and to correlate them with students’ medical performance.

Peer Review reports

Background

It is important for society and for future patients that medical schools accept the challenge to select the ideal people to become tomorrow’s doctors [1]. Medical students’ performance is influenced and can be predicted by personal attributes, which also predict well-being and coping strategies of medical students during the demanding medical training [2]. Knowledge-based medical school entry tests, which are frequently used, seem to select diligent applicants but might miss out on other important personal characteristics important for medical doctors [3]. A medical school admission process that additionally considers desirable personality traits which relate to performance as physicians could be advantageous in the long run [4].

Medical school selection processes which take applicants’ psychological properties into account have already been established at many medical faculties worldwide [5,6,7]. At the Medical Faculty of Hamburg University, the selection process for medical school applicants includes a natural science test (HAM-Nat) in a first step [8] and multiple mini-interviews (HAM-Int) in a second step [9, 10] besides high school grade point averages. We already detected that rejected applicants who participated in the multiple mini-interviews (HAM-Int) showed higher levels of depressive symptoms than admitted applicants [11]. This can be partly explained by maladaptive perfectionism [11], which is an important finding because – in contrast – adaptive perfectionism [12] has been described to be highest in medical school applicants admitted by their grade point average only [13]. Furthermore, adaptive perfectionism shows a general decrease during undergraduate medical studies independent of the admission procedure [14]. Even though adaptive perfectionism, which includes self-oriented perfectionism and personal standards [12], is an important characteristic for medical students and physicians, other personality traits are needed as well, because physicians have to endure and deal with uncertainty on a daily basis when working with patients [15, 16].

The ability to tolerate ambiguity is beneficial for being able to deal with uncertainty [17]. Individuals with lower need for cognitive closure showed increased cognitive effort for higher task ambiguity, while individuals with higher need for cognitive closure showed increased cognitive effort for higher outcome relevance [18]. This suggests that the interaction between need for cognitive closure and context influences perceptual decision making [18]. While tolerance for ambiguity describes an individual’s perception of uncertain situations, the need for cognitive closure identifies an individual’s extent of desire for closure toward ambiguity [19]. Ambiguity can be understood as an event with more than one possible interpretation and frequently occurs in medical contexts such as during history taking, diagnostics, and patient treatment. Each situation carries a varying degree of uncertainty and tolerance for ambiguity focuses on the cognitive interpretation of uncertainty [19]. An individual’s perception of an ambiguous situation can either tend to interpret it as desirable, which describes tolerance for ambiguity, or as a source of threat or discomfort, which describes intolerance of ambiguity [20, 21]. The ability to tolerate ambiguity has been shown to have an impact on physicians’ level of perceiving work-related stress [17]. Low tolerance for ambiguity is associated with the tendency to order more diagnostic tests [17] and the need for cognitive closure hampers medical decision making as well [22, 23]. Furthermore, need for cognitive closure identifies the motivation to approach or avoid closure [19] and describes an individual’s desire for a firm answer to a question and an aversion toward ambiguity [23, 24].

The relevant personal qualities for medical school students who will perform well as future physicians are not completely defined [25]. Hence, studies with medical school applicants are needed to investigate aspects which are relevant for physicians to perform well in their job for a functional health care system and to stay healthy at the same time [2, 26, 27]. Especially personality aspects like perfectionism, ambiguity tolerance, and need for cognitive closure [2, 11, 17, 28] could be desirable targets in medical student selection to measure applicants’ capacity for adapting and coping with uncertainty and decision making. Currently, these personality aspects are not included in our multiple mini-interviews, which measure psychosocial competencies such as empathy, communication skills, and self-regulation [8, 10]. We hypothesize that the expression of these personality aspects could play an additional role as prediction markers in the medical school selection process. Therefore, the aim of our study was to explore possible differences in ambiguity tolerance and in need for cognitive closure and their correlation with aspects of perfectionism between admitted and rejected medical school applicants.

Methods

Study design and participants

The Medical Faculty of Hamburg employs a two-step-process for the selection of medical students. First, medical applicants with a high school grade point average of 1.8 or better (a GPA of 1.0 resembles the best grade, 6.0 the worst grade) are invited to partake in a natural science test (HAM-Nat). The 100 best participants (rank 1 to 100) after the HAM-Nat are admitted to medical school, while the following 200 applicants (rank 101 to 300) are invited to participate in a mulitple mini-intervies selection process (HAM-Int). In August 2019, 196 applicants took part in the HAM-Int and were randomly divided into two groups for procedural reasons. The Hamburg Medical Faculty’s final ranking for medical school admission is based on a sum of the applicants’ GPA, HAM-Nat, and HAM-Int scores. Enrolment of study participants ensued on a voluntary basis during the debriefing phase after the HAM-Int. The questionnaire was administered to the participants who gave their written informed consent and took about 15 min to be completed. A person not involved in this study consolidated relevant admission data (e.g. HAM-Nat, HAM-Int, and GPA scores) as well as basic sociodemographic details such as gender and age with the questionnaire data. The questionnaire contained the following validated instruments: Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale by Hewitt and Flett (MPS-H) [29], Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale by Frost (MPS-F) [30], Tolerance for Ambiguity Scale [20], and the 16-Need for Cognitive Closure Scale [24]. Applicants signed informed consent prior to the admission procedure and the local psychological ethics commission (Lokale Psychologische Ethikkommission am Zentrum für Psychosoziale Medizin – LPEK) approved of this study (reference number: LPEK-0042) under the condition that data were anonymized. We clearly stated that any results of the questionnaire would not affect the HAM-Int score. The final admission decision was still unknown to the participants and the authors of this study at the time when the questionnaire was filled out.

Instruments

Multidimensional perfectionism scale (MPS-H)

The MPS-H consists of three scales, each containing 15 items: Self-oriented perfectionism (SOP), Other-oriented perfectionism (OOP), and Socially-prescribed perfectionism (SPP). Items are rated on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = full disagreement to 7 = full agreement) [29]. We used the German version of this instrument [11, 13] based on Stoeber [31]. The three scales showed satisfactory internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = .88, .84 and .74 respectively).

Multidimensional perfectionism scale (MPS-F)

This instrument consists of a total of 35 items rated on a 6-point Likert scale (1 = being not at all true to 6 = being exactly true) [30] (German version by Altstötter-Gleich and Bergemann [32]). This scale measures six dimensions, i.e. Personal standards (PS, seven items), Organization (O, six items), Concern over mistakes (CM, nine items), Doubts about actions (DA, four items), Parental expectations (PE, five items), and Parental criticism (PC, four items). Five scales showed acceptable to good internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha for O = .92, PE = .87, CM = .84, PS = .75, and PC = .74), while the reliability of the scale DA only showed an internal consistency of .54.

Tolerance for ambiguity scale (TAS)

This scale originates on the measure of tolerance for ambiguity by Herman et al. [20] as an improved variant of the Budner scale [21]. The TAS refined the Budner scale to 12 items covering the four dimensions Valuing diverse others, Change, Challenging perspectives, and Unfamiliarity [20]. The items are scored on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 3 = neither agree nor disagree, 5 = strongly agree). The items were translated into German by JG and the final versions were discussed with SH, SP, and DV. It shows an internal consistency of .68.

16-need for cognitive closure scale (16-NCCS)

The 16-NCCS [24] comprises 16 items and measures the need for cognitive closure. The 16-item NCC scale is an advancement of the 15-item NFC scale [33], which was constructed from an earlier 47-item version to measure the need for closure [23]. The 16-NCCS is valid for the use in German language [24]. The instruments’ internal consistency is .79.

Data processing

Optical mark recognition software (Remark Office OMR, pki Informationssysteme für Marktforschung, Hamburg, Germany) was used to obtain data from the questionnaires. Data were analyzed with a general alpha-level of .05 using IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, version 25 (IBM Corp., Armonk, N.Y., USA). GPA and all scores from the selection process were transformed into a linear scale so that the GPA accounts for 34% of the final score, while HAM-Nat and HAM-Int account for 33%, respectively. HAM-Nat and HAM-Int scores ranged from 0 to 59, while GPA ranged from 0 to 60. We included an individual participant in the data set when at least 80% of the questionnaire items were rated. Imputation of the mean of the scale ensued for remaining missing data. Sum scores from the subscales of the MPS-H as well as from the MPS-F were calculated and z-standardized. The composite measure of adaptive perfectionism (AP) consists of the z-standardized scores of SOP from MPH-S and PS from MPH-F, while Maladaptive Perfectionism (MP) consists of the z-standardized subscales SPP from MPH-S, and CM and DA from MPH-F. Group differences between admitted and rejected applicants were calculated with independent t-tests. Where applicable, Cohen’s d was calculated to estimate effect sizes. The general association of adaptive as well as maladaptive perfectionism, tolerance for ambiguity, and need for cognitive closure as well as their association with the decision measures was assessed using Pearson correlations. We calculated a binary logistic regression model to predict the admission decision with the predictors of need for cognitive closure, age, tolerance for ambiguity, adaptive perfectionism, and maladaptive perfectionism.

Results

Of 196 administered questionnaires, 189 questionnaires were returned. Five of these had to be excluded before data processing due to missing identification and 11 had to be excluded because of missing written consent for data consolidation. A total of 173 questionnaires entered data processing (return rate: 88%). Participants’ characteristics are displayed in Table 1. Mean age of medical school applicants was 19.90 ± 1.63 years, with a minimum age of 17 and a maximum age of 25 years. Mean GPA was 1.51 ± 0.19 (mean GPA score: 49.71 ± 3.77), mean HAM-Nat score was 35.55 ± 4.20, and mean HAM-Int score was 34.82 ± 4.97. Mean total score, on which the applicants’ final ranking was based, was 120.09 ± 5.58. Applicants showed a mean 16-NCCS score of 2.89 ± 0.59, a mean TAS score of 3.55 ± 0.48, a score of .01 ± 1.79 for adaptive perfectionism (AP) and a score of −.02 ± 2.45 for maladaptive perfectionism (MP). Of the 173 applicants, 88 were eventually admitted according to the acceptance quota, while 85 were rejected. Rejected applicants were significantly younger (p = .005, d = 0.43), had significantly lower HAM-Nat scores (p = .013), significantly lower HAM-Int scores (p < .001, d = 1.94), and significantly lower total scores (p < .001, d = 2.47). Rejected applicants showed a significantly higher score on the 16-NCCS (p = .009). No significant differences between rejected and admitted applicants in GPA, GPA score, MPS-H or MPS-F as well as TAS scores were found.

Table 1 Participants’ characteristics

The 16-NCCS correlated highly negatively with the TAS (r = −.57, p < .001), highly positively with MP (r = .51, p < .001), and moderately positively with AP (r = .29, p < .001). The TAS showed a moderate negative association with MP (r = −.29, p < .001), but not with AP. The correlation between admission measures and questionnaires’ variables only showed a significant weak positive correlation between HAM-Int and 16-NCCS (r = .182, p = .017) (Table 2). The logistic regression model with the variables need for cognitive closure, tolerance for ambiguity, adaptive perfectionism, maladaptive perfectionism and age (Table 3) was statistically significant (χ2(5) = 17.81, p = .003). The model correctly classified 57% of cases and explained 13.5% (Nagelkerke’s R2) of the variance in admission decisions. Older age and lower need for cognitive closure were associated with an increased likelihood of a positive admission decision. Tolerance for ambiguity, adaptive perfectionism or maladaptive perfectionism did not significantly predict the admission decision in the logistic regression model.

Table 2 Correlation of perfectionism, need for cognitive closure, tolerance for ambiguity, GPA, HAM-Nat, and HAM-Int
Table 3 Binary logistic regression analysis for prediction of admission decision

Discussion

Multiple mini-interviews are designed to select applicants with personality traits which are supposed to predict that they will become “good doctors” [7]. Rejected applicants who participated in our multiple mini-interview medical school selection procedure had a higher need for cognitive closure compared to admitted applicants. A higher need for cognitive closure can lead to lower decision quality in medical practice [34] and seems to be a less desirable personality trait for physicians. Therefore, the need for cognitive closure could be measured during the selection process as additional prediction marker for desired applicants. Individuals with lower need for cognitive closure invest more cognitive effort when tasks become more difficult [18], seem to be more desirable to be selected for medical studies because physicians have to deal with increasing difficulty of tasks on a daily basis.

Furthermore, in our study, a higher need for cognitive closure, which we predominantly found in rejected applicants, was related to lower tolerance for ambiguity. However, in physicians’ diagnostic processes, high tolerance for ambiguity is needed – and therefore desirable in medical school applicants, because the majority of patients present with complaints that cannot be instantly classified as distinct disease entities [35]. Furthermore, low tolerance of ambiguity is related to lower tolerance of uncertainty [36], which also frequently occurs in medical practice and needs to be handled professionally. Hence, an aversion towards ambiguity [37] is not supportive for future physicians’ daily work and does not seem desirable in medical school applicants. Hence, measuring the tolerance of ambiguity in medical school applicants could be an interesting additional aspect in the selection process to predict suitable applicants for undergraduate medical studies.

Additionally, higher levels of intrinsic motivation have been linked to lower levels of need for cognitive closure and vice versa [18]. In our study, maladaptive perfectionism, which is composed of the subscales Socially-prescribed perfectionism, Concern over mistakes, and Doubts about action [12], was also related to higher need for cognitive closure which confirms these findings and does not seem to be a desirable personality trait for medical students or physicians. Furthermore, we could show in a previous study that undergraduate medical students with higher levels of maladaptive perfectionism showed higher scores for symptoms of depression and anxiety [13]. These findings suggest, that measuring the different aspects of perfectionism in addition to the need for cognitive closure and the tolerance of ambiguity could support finding the desired medical school applicants in the selection process.

Our binary logistic regression model showed lower need for cognitive closure and older applicants’ age as main predictors for the admission decision. The admission measures seem to assess the constructs we investigated in this study, but the bivariate correlation only revealed the 16-NCCS as weakly correlating with the multiple mini-interviews. The students’ age seemed to have the main impact on admission measures. However, students’ age should not be a discriminatory factor for admission decisions. Interestingly, older age was associated with higher need for cognitive closure in another study but the efficacy of fulfilling the need for cognitive closure decreased with aging [38]. Hence, the higher need for cognitive closure in older individuals does not necessarily result in the same closed behaviour found by younger individuals with need for cognitive closure [39]. Furthermore, the need for cognitive closure level of an individual is only consistent with the individual’s behaviour under positive mood [40]. These findings show that measuring an individual’s need for cognitive closure alone might not be sufficient to predict a person’s actual behaviour. A higher level of tolerance for ambiguity and perfectionism was found in individuals above 25 years of age [2]. Interestingly, ambiguity aversion, which positively correlates with the need for cognitive closure [24], also increased with older age [37]. Being able to have high tolerance for ambiguity helped medical students to cope with uncertainty [41] and was also shown to play a role in medical school selection [42].

A strength of our study is the differentiated view on medical school applicants for dealing with uncertainty by studying the combined expressions of need for cognitive closure, tolerance for ambiguity, and perfectionism in a large cohort of medical school admitted and rejected applicants who participated in a multiple mini-interview admission procedure. Both perfectionism scales showed good internal consistency and the results were similar to our previous study with another cohort of medical school applicants [11]. The 16-NCCS showed an acceptable internal consistency of .79. A limitation of our study is the TAS’ low reliability of .68. This could be due to item translation without additional validation of the translated questionnaire. Another limitation is that this study was only performed at one institution and that only a single admission process was observed with one cohort of applicants. However, our findings still provide a first assumption of association between these characteristics in medical school applicants, which need to be further investigated.

Our results underscore that measuring the need for cognitive closure alone and possibly rejecting medical school applicants with a higher need for cognitive closure, because it can lead to lower decision quality in medical practice [34], could comprise the risk of neglecting useful abilities in individuals with higher need for cognitive closure [18], which could be needed in certain medical specialties. Measuring the different aspects of perfectionism in combination with the need for cognitive closure in medical school applicants like in our study provides a possibility to identify the status of applicants’ motivation. Since motivation is related to mood [43] and the level of need for cognitive closure and an individual’s actual behaviour are consistent under positive mood [40], an additional test for medical school applicants’ mood might help to predict their future behaviour as physicians. Similar to a study in physicians [17] it might be useful to test medical students with respect to these personality traits longitudinally to assist them in developing their personal potential. Since higher need for cognitive closure leads to a preference to consider smaller amounts of information before making final decisions [44], which might hamper decision-making processes in the long run, teaching strategies and exercises need to be explored that reduce the need for cognitive closure [45].

Conclusions

Our study shows different personality aspects with respect to the need for cognitive closure in medical school applicants participating in a multiple mini-interview selection test. Need for cognitive closure was significantly higher in rejected applicants and higher need for cognitive closure was related to maladaptive perfectionism and to lower tolerance for ambiguity. A lower score of need for cognitive closure was a main predictor for the admission decision in combination with older age. With respect to medical decision-making of physicians in an environment of great uncertainty, the admission of applicants with a profile of low need for cognitive closure, high tolerance for ambiguity, and low levels of maladaptive perfectionism seems desirable. Since an individuals’ need for cognitive closure can be modulated by their ability to achieve cognitive closure, the latter seems to be an additional important measure. To decide whether testing medical school applicants for these different personality traits could be useful or whether different admission selection procedures already implicitly select students with respect to these measures, further research is necessary. Longitudinal studies in admitted medical students for such characteristics would additionally be interesting to monitor their possible changes and to combine the levels of their expression with medical students’ performance.

Availability of data and materials

All data and materials are available from the manuscript and from the corresponding author upon request.

Abbreviations

AP:

Adaptive perfectionism

CM:

Concern over mistakes

DA:

Doubts about actions

GPA:

High school grade point average

HAM-Int:

Hamburg Assessment Test for Medicine – Interview

HAM-Nat:

Hamburg Assessment Test for Medicine – Natural Science Test

MP:

Maladaptive perfectionism

MPS-F:

Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale by Frost

MPS-H:

Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale by Hewitt and Flett

NFC:

Need for cognitive closure

16-NCCS:

Need for Cognitive Closure Scale

O:

Organization

OOP:

Other-oriented perfectionism

PC:

Parental criticism

PE:

Parental expectations

PS:

Personal standards

SOP:

Self-oriented perfectionism

SPP:

Socially-prescribed perfectionism

TAS:

Tolerance for Ambiguity Scale

References

  1. Morrison J. Selecting for medical education. Med Educ. 2016;50(1):3–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Leung J, Cloninger CR, Hong BA, Cloninger KM, Eley DS. Temperament and character profiles of medical students associated with tolerance of ambiguity and perfectionism. PeerJ. 2019;7:e7109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Abbiati M, Baroffio A, Gerbase MW. Personal profile of medical students selected through a knowledge-based exam only: are we missing suitable students? Med Educ Online. 2016;21:29705.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Schripsema NR, van Trigt AM, van der Wal MA, Cohen-Schotanus J. How different medical school selection processes call upon different personality characteristics. PLoS One. 2016;11(3):e0150645.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Eva KW, Rosenfeld J, Reiter HI, Norman GR. An admissions OSCE: the multiple mini-interview. Med Educ. 2004;38(3):314–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Pau A, Jeevaratnam K, Chen YS, Fall AA, Khoo C, Nadarajah VD. The multiple mini-interview (MMI) for student selection in health professions training – a systematic review. Med Teach. 2013;35(12):1027–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Knorr M, Hissbach J. Multiple mini-interviews: same concept, different approaches. Med Educ. 2014;48(12):1157–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Hissbach JC, Klusmann D, Hampe W. Dimensionality and predictive validity of the HAM-Nat, a test of natural sciences for medical school admission. BMC Med Educ. 2011;11(1):83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Hissbach JC, Sehner S, Harendza S, Hampe W. Cutting costs of multiple mini-interviews – changes in reliability and efficiency of the Hamburg medical school admission test between two applications. BMC Med Educ. 2014;14(1):54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Knorr M, Schwibbe A, Ehrhardt M, Lackamp J, Zimmermann S, Hampe W. Validity evidence for the Hamburg multiple mini-interview. BMC Med Educ. 2018;18(1):106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Bußenius L, Harendza S. The relationship between perfectionism and symptoms of depression in medical school applicants. BMC Med Educ. 2019;19(1):370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Enns MW, Cox BJ, Sareen J, Freeman P. Adaptive and maladaptive perfectionism in medical students: a longitudinal investigation. Med Educ. 2001;35(11):1034–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Seeliger H, Harendza S. Is perfect good? – dimensions of perfectionism in newly admitted medical students. BMC Med Educ. 2017;17(1):206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Vogel D, Seeliger H, Harendza S. Longitudinal development of different dimensions of perfectionism in undergraduate medical students with resect to their medical school admission procedure. GMS J Med Educ. 2019;36(4):Doc44.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Kim K, Lee YM. Understanding uncertainty in medicine: concepts and implications in medical education. Med Educ. 2018;30(3):181–8.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Coles C. Uncertainty in a world of regulation. Adv Psychiatr Treat. 2006;12(6):397–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Ianello P, Mottini A, Tirelli S, Riva S, Antonietti A. Ambiguity and uncertainty tolerance, need for cognition, and their association with stress. A study among Italian practicing physicians. Med Educ Online. 2017;22(1):1270009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Viola V, Tosoni A, Brizi A, Salvato I, Kruglanski AW, et al. Need for cognitive closure modulates how perceptual decisions are affected by task difficulty and outcome relevance. PLoS One. 2015;10(2):e0146002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Rosen NO, Knäuper EI, Knäuper B. Differentiation intolerance of uncertainty from three related but distinct constructs. Anxiety Stress Coping. 2015;27(1):55–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Herman JL, Stevens MJ, Bird A, Mendenhall M, Oddou G. The tolerance for ambiguity scale: towards a more refined measure for international management research. IJIR. 2010;34(1):58–65.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Budner S. Intolerance of ambiguity as a personality variable. J Pers. 1962;30(1):29–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Roets A, Raman E, Heytens S, Avonts D. Effects of dispositional need for cognitive closure and training on medical decision making. Med Decis Mak. 2013;34(2):144–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Kruglanski AW, Webster DM, Klem A. Motivated resistance and openness to persuasion in the presence or absence of prior information. JPSP. 1993;65(5):861.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Schlink S, Walther E. Kurz und gut: Eine deutsche Kurzskala zur Erfassung des Bedürfnisses nach kognitiver Geschlossenheit [short and sweet: a German short scale to measure need for cognitive closure]. Z Sozialpsychol. 2007;38(3):153–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Lynagh M, Horton G, Nair BK, Walker M, Kelly B, Powis D. Student selection for medicine: still a “thorny” issue. Arch Med Health Sci. 2018;6:147–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Wallace JE, Lemaire JB, Ghali WA. Physician wellness: a missing quality indicator. Lancet. 2009;374(9702):1714–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Cohen D, Winstanley S, Palmer P, Allen J, Howells S, et al. Factors that impact on medical student wellbeing – perspectives of risk: Individual Support Programme School of Medicine Cardiff University; 2012. Available from: www.aamc.org/download/321442/data/factstable1.pdf. [Accessed: 31 Oct 2019].

  28. Kosic A. Need for cognitive closure and coping strategies. Int J Psychol. 2002;37(1):35–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Hewitt PL, Flett GL. Multidimensional perfectionism scale (MPS): technical manual. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems; 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Frost RO, Marten P, Lahart C, Rosenblate R. The dimensions of perfectionism. Cognit Ther Res. 1990;14(5):449–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Stoeber J. In: Dalbert C, editor. Skalendokumentation “Persönliche Ziele von SchülerInnen” [scale documentation “personal goals of high school students”]. In: Hallesche Berichte zur Pädagogischen Psychologie. 3rd ed. Halle (Saale): Martin Luther University; 2002.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Altstötter-Gleich C, Bergemann N. Testgüte einer deutschsprachigen version der mehrdimensionalen Perfektionismusskala von Frost, Marten, Lahart und Rosenblate (MPS-F) [the statistics and validity of German version of a multidimensional perfectionism scale of Frost, Marten, Lahart and Rosenblate (MPS-F)]. Diagnostica. 2006;52(3):105–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Roets A, Van Hiel A. Item selection and validation of a brief, 15-item version of the need for closure scale. Personal Individ Differ. 2011;50(1):90–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Raglan GB, Babush M, Farrow VA, Kruglansky AW, Schulkin J. Need to know: the need for cognitive closure impacts the clinical practice of obstetrician/gynecologists. BMC Med Inform Decis Mak. 2014;14:122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Norman GR, Eva KW. Diagnostic error and clinical reasoning. Med Educ. 2010;44(1):94–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Kossowska M, Jasko K, Bar-Tal Y. Need for closure and cognitive structuring among younger and older adults. Pol Psychol Bull. 2012;43(1):40–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Han PKJ, Reeve BB, Moser RP, Klein WMP. Aversion to ambiguity regarding medical tests and treatments: measurement, prevalence, and relationship to sociodemographic factors. J Health Commun. 2009;14(6):556–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Buhr K, Dugas MJ. Investigating the construct validity of intolerance of uncertainty and its unique relationship with worry. J Anxiety Disord. 2006;20(2):222–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Roets A, Kruglanski AW, Kossowska M, Pierro A, Hong Y. The motivation gatekeeper of our minds. Adv Exp Soc Psychol. 2015;52:221–83.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Kossowska M, Bar-Tal Y. Positive mood boosts the expression of a dipositional need for closure. Cognit Emot. 2013;27(7):1181–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Merrill JM, Camacho Z, Laux LF, Lorimor R, Thornby JI, Vallbona C. Uncertainties and ambiguities: measuring how medical students cope. Med Educ. 1994;28(4):316–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Geller G, Faden RR, Levine DM. Tolerance for ambiguity among medical students: implications for their selection, training and practice. Soc Sci Med. 1990;31(5):619–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Gendolla GHE. On the impact of mood on behaviour: an integrative theory and a review. Rev Gen Psychol. 2000;4(4):378–408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Choi JA, Koo M, Coi I, Auh S. Need for cognitive closure and information search strategy. Psychol Mark. 2008;25:1027–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Djikic M, Oatley K, Moldoveanu MC. The effect of exporsure to literature on the need for closure. Creat Res J. 2013;25(2):149–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank all medical school applicants who participated in this study.

Funding

This work was supported by the Federal Ministry of Education and Research (project number: 01GK1801A). The funding body played no role in the design of the study and in collection, analysis, and interpretation of data and in writing the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

JG, SH, SP, and DV planned the study and constructed the questionnaires. LB acquired the data and performed the statistical analysis. JG, LB and SH drafted the manuscript and all authors approved its final version.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sigrid Harendza.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The Local Psychological Ethics Commission at the Center for Psychosocial Medicine at the University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf (Lokale Psychologische Ethikkommission am Zentrum für Psychosoziale Medizin - LPEKconfirmed the innocuousness of this study (reference number: LPEK-0042). Informed consent was obtained by all participants of this study in written form and the anonymity of all participants has been guaranteed.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

SH has a position as Section Editor to BMC Medical Education. JG, LB, SP and DV have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gärtner, J., Bußenius, L., Prediger, S. et al. Need for cognitive closure, tolerance for ambiguity, and perfectionism in medical school applicants. BMC Med Educ 20, 132 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12909-020-02043-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12909-020-02043-2

Keywords