Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Differences in procedural knowledge after a “spaced” and a “massed” version of an intensive course in emergency medicine, investigating a very short spacing interval

Abstract

Background

Distributing a fixed amount of teaching hours over a longer time period (spaced approach) may result in better learning than delivering the same amount of teaching within a shorter time (massed approach). While a spaced approach may provide more opportunities to elaborate the learning content, a massed approach allows for more economical utilisation of teaching facilities and to optimise time resources of faculty. Favourable effects of spacing have been demonstrated for postgraduate surgery training and for spacing intervals of weeks to months. It is however unknown, whether a spacing effect can also be observed for shorter intervals and in undergraduate medical education. Therefore, we aimed to evaluate the effect of a short spacing intervention within an undergraduate intensive course in emergency medicine (EM) on students’ procedural knowledge.

Methods

An EM intensive course of 26 teaching hours was delivered over either 4.5 days, or 3.0 days. After the course students’ procedural knowledge was assessed by a specifically developed video-case based key-feature test (KF-test).

Results

Data sets of 156 students (81.7 %, 191 students eligible) were analysed, 54 from the spaced, and 102 from the massed version. In the KF-test students from the spaced version reached a mean of 14.8 (SD 2.0) out of 22 points, compared to 13.7 (SD 2.0) in the massed version (p = .002). Effect size was moderate (Cohen’s d: 0.558).

Conclusion

A significant spacing effect was observable even for a short spacing interval in undergraduate medical education. This effect was only moderate and may be weighed against planning needs of faculty and teaching resources.

Peer Review reports

Background

Curricular planning should combine best possible delivery of learning content with most efficient consumption of resources including time, facilities, equipment, and personnel. Under this perspective it is not clear, whether distribution of a fixed amount of teaching hours over a longer time period (‘spaced approach’) is more efficient than over a shorter time period (‘massed approach’). Arguments in favour of a spaced approach are based on learning theory. It is well acknowledged that learning content needs to be transferred to long-term memory [1, 2]. This encoding process needs opportunities for rehearsal and repetition, preferentially in self-regulated learning approaches [35]. A spaced course concept would provide more time for such kinds of elaboration [68]. Another mechanism of memory consolidation is sleep. Spaced distribution of learning may allow for more phases of dreaming enabling sleep-dependent memory consolidation [911], especially if the content is expected to be relevant for future retrieval [12]. Advantages of spaced learning have been supported by neurophysiological MRI-studies, where knowledge gain was more sustainable, if learning had been distributed over more time [13].

On the other side, advantages of a massed teaching approach primarily fall in the field of resource planning. A more economical use of facilities and equipment may be accomplished, if scarce rooms and complex technical equipment can be utilised for more hours per day. In emergency medicine (EM), where sophisticated and costly simulators and manikins are used for teaching and assessment, economic considerations become relevant. An additional advantage of a massed approach could be the opportunity for teaching personnel to concentrate their teaching activities on specific time periods in order to be set free for clinical or scientific work on other days. Also, from many students’ perspective a massed course version is perceived as beneficial, as it leaves opportunities for learning of other subjects in the remaining time. This would serve self-regulated learning concepts in a broader way than just considering one single subject [5]. Finally, in terms of curricular planning, a massed approach could help to maintain a more consistent course structure, avoiding unintended redundancy between different teachers.

For teaching EM, intensive course formats are typical with rotating blocks of small group scenario teaching and the use of sophisticated technical equipment. It is of considerable interest for this format how to fit teaching hours into a rotation plan.

Differences between spaced and massed course formats have already been studied in the field of surgical skills training for residents [1416]. In postgraduate training significant advantages could be shown for the spaced approach [15] but an influence of the pattern of distribution was not revealed [14]. Residency training however, may not be comparable to undergraduate training since the nature of the task plays an important role for the effect of spacing [17] and learning motivation is clearly different during residency, as is the presence of plenty of opportunities for (deliberate) practice. Accordingly, an investigation within an undergraduate paediatric resuscitation training found only slightly superior performance in the “spaced” student group [18]. The intervention itself was as short as 5 teaching hrs, while the difference in distribution was 1.25 h once a week vs. 5 h on 1 day.

In our specific setting, we wanted to study the effect of a much smaller spacing interval for a longer teaching intervention within an undergraduate EM course: 26 teaching hours were distributed either of over 4.5 days, or 3.0 days (see Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Study principle: 26 teaching hours in 4.5 days vs. 3.0 days (areas of both concepts equal each other)

This relatively small difference between the two versions represented a typical real life question of curriculum design, with the rationale to utilise teaching resources more efficiently. The chosen interval may be regarded as very short compared to spacing intervals usually studied under experimental conditions [6], however still distinctly different from a massed version. Based on previous literature, we hypothesised that the teaching intervention of 26 teaching hours was strong enough to produce a relevant effect.

The primary outcome we were aiming at was performance closely related to clinical work. Thus we wanted to measure procedural knowledge and decision making in order to reach higher cognitive levels than by traditional multiple choice questionnaire (MCQ). We therefore chose a key-feature test (KF-test), which focuses its questions to “critical step [s] in the resolution of a problem” [1922]. The KF-test in addition provided the opportunity to relate assessment to patient outcome. Furthermore, we wanted the test to represent real-life decisions in a better fashion than text vignettes, and therefore based the questions on sequential video-cases. By including visual information and dynamic time courses the test was also less dependent on pure reading competencies.

As primary endpoint we defined the difference in overall KF-test score, with the hypothesis that scores would be higher in the spaced course version. As a secondary endpoint we hypothesised that students within the spaced course version would spend more time on additional elaborative learning (additional individual learning time and exchange with co-students).

Methods

This prospective study was conducted at the Charité-Universitätsmedizin Berlin (Germany) during the winter term of 2011/2012. The EM intensive course was part of the fifth year within a 6 year undergraduate curriculum, directly preceding the clinical elective year (CEY). Primary goal was to prepare students for medical emergencies during CEY.

All students were assigned to one of the two course versions according to the standard rotation plan for all clinical courses of the term. The EM course included 26 teaching hours (of 45 min each), which were spread over either 4.5 days, or 3.0 days. Courses of the spaced version were distributed over a whole week (4.5 days), leaving each afternoon to the students for free time, or self-regulated learning. Course assessment was performed at the end of the fifth morning. Aim of the massed approach was to fit three courses into a 2 week period, leaving the last day of the 2 weeks for assessment of all students. These organisational issues caused that massed courses had different time spans between course and assessment day (0, 3, 8 days), see Fig. 2. Therefore, we also compared test results between these sub-groups in order to estimate a potential influence of this difference.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Study design: Distribution of 26 teaching hours (45 min encounter) of an intensive course in emergency medicine over 4.5 days (spaced version, light grey boxes), and 3.0 days (massed version, dark grey boxes). The massed course version was delivered with three different time intervals between course and assessment: 8 days, 3 days, and 0 days. KF-Test: “Key-Feature test”

The KF-test was taken on a voluntary basis, scheduled directly before the official summative assessment of the course.

Emergency medicine (EM) course

Learning objectives were identical for both course versions and were known to students and teachers in advance. Teachers were highly experienced EM physicians with teaching experience for at least 5 years. The main teaching format was case-based scenario teaching using manikins and EM equipment, teacher-student ratio was 1:5 to 1:6. Course content was based on the 2010 guidelines of the European Resuscitation Council [23]. Topics included: dyspnea/respiratory failure; airway management; acute chest pain; gastrointestinal emergencies/acute abdomen; neurological emergencies (stroke, epilepsy, intoxication); severe injuries/polytrauma; forms of shock.

Key-feature test (KF-Test)

Seven sequential video cases were designed in accordance to the learning objectives. They covered the following clinical problems: (1) cardiac arrest, (2) hypoglycaemia, (3) exacerbation of asthma, (4) cardiac arrhythmia, (5) polytrauma following road accident, (6) anaphylactic shock, (7) impaired consciousness of unknown origin. Videos were embedded into an electronic test format, which posed a total of 63 questions, 22 of which were decisions directly related to patient safety or outcome. Only these 22 critical questions were included in the test score in order to provide a direct link to patient safety (e.g. “administer oxygen” for a patient in asthmatic status).

Technically, the KF-test was programmed with a “Survey Monkey”™ software (https://de.surveymonkey.com), securing the testing environment against the possibility to return to previous questions. The testing environment also enabled the examinee to return to the correct clinical path after false decisions (as it would happen in a real clinical situation). The programme was run on a secured browser application, disabling all notebook functions apart from those keys, which were necessary for test completion (“Safe Exam Browser”™, http://safeexambrowser.org).

At the end of the test participants reported basic demographic data, relevant previous experience in emergency medical services (EMS), and learning strategies in preparation of and during the EM course (total additional learning, type of learning media used, verbal exchange with others, overnight dreaming of learning content). Participants gave their permission to use the anonymous data set. The test was designed for an answering time of 45–60 min.

Selection of participants

The course was held at three different sites of the campus, so we had to use a mobile set of 20 notebooks. This made it impossible to assess all students who were enrolled in the course. For testing we selected those time slots at which the largest number of students was expected to participate in the voluntary KF-test. Two sessions at the beginning of the term were used to test the technical environment. We defined all students as eligible for the KF-test who were scheduled for regular course assessment at time slots, at which we had the technical environment to perform KF-testing. Distribution of cohorts followed the routine process at the office of students affairs, without formal randomisation. For the final analysis all data sets of students were included who had fully attended one of the two course versions; students participating in academic exchange programmes (e.g. ERASMUS) were excluded (see Fig. 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Flow chart: Included and excluded students

Ethics, consent and permissions

Results of the KF-test, demographic data, and information on learning strategies were stored as complete data sets. No information was collected, which could have identified single students. The study was approved by the ethical committee of Charité-Universitätsmedizin Berlin (EA1/326/14).

Statistical analysis

Raw data were transferred to a spread sheet data file. Metric parameters were de-scribed by mean and standard deviation (SD). Significance was assumed at p < .05. The Institute of Biometry of Charité-Universitätsmedizin Berlin performed the analysis (using SPSS 19.0.0). Sample size consideration was based on a “clinically” relevant effect of a 10 % difference in the KF-test score. To reach a power of 0.90 with an alpha-error of 0.05 and a beta-error of 0.80, a minimum sample size of 68 students per group was calculated.

Mann–Whitney test was used to compare KF-test scores between groups. Comparison of students’ characteristics, learning media, and learning times was performed by chi-square test. For internal test consistency, Cronbach’s alpha was calculated, for effect size Cohen’s d was calculated.

Results

Students’ characteristics

Of 191 eligible students, 164 participated in the KF-test (85.9 %). 156 complete data sets could be included in the analysis (81.7 %), 54 from students of the spaced course version, and 102 from the massed version (see Fig. 3). There were no statistical differences between the course versions for drop-outs, gender distribution, German as native language, or previous experience in EMS, only significantly more students of the massed group were younger than 25 years. Details are given in Table 1. Sub-groups of the massed course version with different time spans between course and KF-test did not show any differences (Additional file 1: Table S1).

Table 1 Characteristics of student groups

KF-test scores

Internal consistency was calculated as Cronbach’s alpha of 0.63. Students from the spaced group reached a mean of 14.8 points (SD 2.0) from a maximum of 22 points, students from the massed group reached 13.7 points (SD 2.0) (see Fig. 4). The difference was statistically highly significant (p = .002). Effect size Cohen’s d was 0.56, which is a moderate effect (0.45 to 0.75, according to Cohen [24]).

Fig. 4
figure 4

KF-test scores of “spaced” student group (light grey) vs. “massed” student group (dark grey); significant difference: p = .002 (Mann–Whitney-U); Cohen’s d: 0.558

Influence of time span between course and KF-test

The massed group was also analysed for differences between the three sub-groups with different time spans between course and assessment. No statistical differences were found (time span of 8 days: mean 13.0 pts (SD 2.7); 3 days: 14.0 pts (SD 2.5); 0 days: 13.7 pts (SD 2.2), see Additional file 2: Figure S1).

Influence of other potential confounders

Gender: In the spaced group, female students (n = 33) performed significantly better than males (n = 21). Mean test scores were 15.2 pts (SD 2.4) vs. 14.1 pts (SD 2.0); p = .04. For the massed group, no statistical difference was found (females: n = 61; 13.7 pts (SD 2.2); males: n = 41; 13.6 pts (SD 2.4); p = .73).

Age: In the massed group, students aged below 25 years (n = 58) reached a significantly higher mean test score (13.9 pts, SD 2.0), compared to students above 25 years (n = 44) (13.3 pts, SD 2.0); p = .04. In the spaced group scores were 15.5 pts (SD 2.1) below an age of 25 years (n = 19) vs. 14.4 pts (SD 2.2) with an age above 25 years (n = 35). This difference did not reach statistical significance (p = .11).

An influence of German as native language could not be shown. Non-native speakers (n = 28) scored at a mean of 14.0 pts (SD 2.0), and native speakers (n = 125) at 14.1 pts (SD 2.0); p = .91.

Learning strategies outside the course

Students of the spaced group reported a course preparation time of “up to 3 h” in 61.1 % of the cases, compared to 61.8 % of the massed group.

Additional learning time during the course period was reported with “up to 2 h” in 70.5 % by the spaced group, compared to 77.5 % by the massed group (“more than 2 h” in 29.5 %, and 22.5 % respectively). Within sub-groups of the massed group replies were similar. Due to the semi-quantitative question format no statistical tests were calculated.

Strategies used for additional learning were reported as shown in Table 2. There were no differences in respect to eLearning, textbooks, or practical training. However, significant differences in favour of the spaced group were found for verbal exchange with co-students and overnight dreaming of learning contents.

Table 2 Additional learning time and strategies, used outside the course time

Discussion

Mean KF-test score-the primary endpoint of this study-was significantly higher in the spaced course version, at a moderate effect size. This finding supports the primary hypothesis. However, this result only transfers into an absolute difference of 1.1 answers, which is 8.6 % of the mean test score of 14.0 pts. If taking into account, that only questions were included, which addressed vital decisions related to patient outcome, and based on the assumption that case-related contextual knowledge was assessed, clinical relevance may have just been reached.

If taking into account, that spacing effects found in postgraduate training [1416] may not be comparable to the undergraduate situation [17, 25], it is remarkable to find an effect with a short spacing interval. In comparison to Patocka’s study in a 5 h undergraduate training [18], the present intervention of 26 h could be regarded as much stronger which might explain the significant results even at an interval as short as an additional 50 % of the massed control group.

One could argue contrary to this explanation, that the difference found could be due to cognitive load which was too high in the massed group and therefore led to lower scores. Although this cannot be ruled out, students still reported substantial time for additional learning outside the course. Nevertheless, there may be an overlap between theories explaining spacing and cognitive load.

It finally remains uncertain, in-how-far the observed differences transfer into practice. Nevertheless, results provide a pragmatic basis to weigh a decision between spaced and massed formats in an intensive course in EM.

KF-test quality

We considered the Cronbach’s alpha of 0.63 to be acceptable, if compared to other reports on KF-tests. Fischer et al. reached a value of 0.65 in an electronically based KF-test of 90 min [19]. Page reported a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.49 [22], and Hatala a value of 0.49 for a 2-h KF-test in postgraduate training [20]. Content validity was regarded to be sufficient, since all relevant fields of EM were covered by the cases. Further support for the validity of the test may be taken from the finding, that female students performed better than males and that younger students reached better scores than older ones. In respect to testing time, it was assumed that more than 45–60 min would not have been acceptable for a voluntary task, even if internal consistency might slightly have been improved. It is possible, that factor analysis would have added another perspective of validity; this will be explored in future projects.

Pre-existing experience in EMS could not be shown to have an influence. However, students in this study had already completed most of their undergraduate training, and therefore experience from the time before their studies may have been less relevant. In addition, the sub-group with such experience was rather small.

As point of interest, students who stated that German was not their native language did not perform worse. Perhaps this finding is due to the video-based test format, which depends less on language comprehension.

Additional learning strategies outside the course

In respect to course preparation no differences were found between the two groups. For additional learning during the EM course a trend could be shown in the direction of more learning time spent in the spaced group (without statistical test). Furthermore, no differences were found between the three massed sub-groups with different time periods between course and day of testing. Taking these findings into account, formal additional learning in the setting did not seem to be influenced by the distribution of course time. It is more likely, that learning was mainly motivated by the (summative) final test at the end of the intensive course. However, interpretations remain speculative.

In respect to learning media, students reported a low amount of practical rehearsal. This can be explained by the fact that the courses themselves provided much hands-on-time and students perceived rather cognitive demands for rehearsal. No differences were reported for any formal learning (skills lab, textbooks, eLearning). However, differences were present for informal learning, such as verbal exchange with co-students, and overnight dreaming. This could slightly indicate deeper learning processes in the spaced course version.

Comparability of study groups

Groups were comparable for all measured confounding variables, except from a greater percentage of younger students in the massed group. Correction for this factor might have led to an even more pronounced advantage of the spaced approach.

Limitations

This study faces a number of limitations. Results are confined to a single centre, to the format of an intensive course, and to the subject of EM. In other contexts, culture of teaching and learning may well be different. As an example, EM is perceived to be highly important by most medical students with the result that many students even dreamed of course contents. This would not be expected from e.g. microbiology. In addition, the KF-test was taken voluntarily; if all eligible students had participated, test scores might have been different. However, drop-out rates were only moderate and were similar for both groups.

The KF-test was taken right at the end of the EM course and therefore neither reflects retention nor transfer into practice. It may well be, that the differences found did not prove to be sustainable. On the other hand, theory implies that differences increase over time in favour of the spaced group [13, 15, 17, 25]. Unfortunately it was not possible to reach the student cohort for follow-up measurements, as they left the university for their final clinical electives at various locations.

Generalisability

Due to the special circumstances of EM, it is unclear, whether results can be transferred to other medical subjects. However, the study context might serve as a model for undergraduate practical training with relevance for critical decision making.

Conclusion

For an undergraduate intensive course in emergency medicine, a significant spacing effect was shown after a relatively short spacing interval. Spaced distribution of teaching sessions (26 teaching hours in 4.5 days) resulted in significantly higher scores in a key-feature test than a massed distribution (26 teaching hours in 3.0 days). However, the difference of 8.6 % of the mean test score is only moderate, as is the effect size of Cohen’s d = 0.56. Additional learning time outside of the course was not statistically different between the two course versions, although there are indicators of increased collaborative exchange and cognitive processing of learning content. Results may help to weigh a decision between promotion of learning and economical curricular planning.

Abbreviations

CEY:

Clinical elective year

EM:

Emergency medicine

EMS:

Emergency medical services

KF-test:

Key-feature-test

MCQ:

Multiple choice questionnaire

SD:

Standard deviation

References

  1. Bahrick HP. Long-term maintenance of knowledge. In: Tulving E, Craik FIM, editors. The Oxford handbook of memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2000. p. 347–62.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Cohen N, Pell L, Edelson MG, Ben-Yakov A, Pine A, Dudai Y. Peri-encoding predictors of memory encoding and consolidation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015;50:128–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Bransford JD, Brown AL, Cocking RR. How people learn. Washington: National Academy Press; 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Paris SG, Byrnes JP, Paris AH. Constructing theories, identities, and actions of self-regulated learners. In: Zimmerman B, Schunk D, editors. Self-regulated learning and academic achievement. New York: Springer; 2001. p. 253–87.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Zimmerman BJ, Schunk D. Self-regulated learning and academic achievement. New York: Springer; 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Cepeda NJ, Pashler H, Vul E, Wixted JT, Rohrer D. Distributed practice in verbal recall tasks: A review and quantitative synthesis. Psychol Bulletin. 2006;132:354–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Seabrook R, Brown GDA, Solity JE. Distributed and massed practice: from laboratory to classroom. Appl Cognit Psychol. 2005;19:107–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Shebilske WL, Goettl BP, Corrington K, Day EA. Inter-lesson spacing and task-related processing during complex skill acquisition. J Exp Psychol Appl. 1999;5:413–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Stickgold R, Hobson JA, Fosse R, Fosse M. Sleep, learning, and dreams: Off-line memory reprocessing. Science. 2001;294:1052–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Walker MP, Stickgold R. Overnight alchemy: Sleep-dependent memory evolution. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11:218–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Wamsley EJ, Tucker M, Payne JD, Benavides JA, Stickgold R. Dreaming of a learning task is associated with enhanced sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Curr Biol. 2010;20:850–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Wilhelm I, Diekelmann S, Molzow I, Ayoub A, Mölle M, Born J. Sleep selectively enhances memory expected to be of future relevance. J Neurosci. 2011;31:1563–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Rohrer D, Pashler H. Recent research on human learning challenges conventional instructional strategies. Educ Res. 2010;39:406–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Mitchell EL, Lee DY, Sevdalis N, Partsafas AW, Landry GJ, Liem TK, Moneta GL. Evaluation of distributed practice schedules on retention of a newly acquired surgical skill: a randomized trial. Am J Surg. 2011;201:31–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Moulton CA, Dubrowski A, Macrae H, Graham B, Grober E, Reznick R. Teaching surgical skills: what kind of practice makes perfect? A randomized, controlled trial. Ann Surg. 2006;244:400–9.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Spruit EN, Band GP, Hamming JF. Increasing efficiency of surgical training: effects of spacing practice on skill acquisition and retention in laparoscopy training. Surg Endosc. 2015;29(8):2235–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Donovan JJ, Radosevich DJ. A meta-analytic review of the distribution of practice effect: Now you see it, now you don’t. J Appl Psychology. 1999;84:795–805.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Patocka C, Khan F, Dubrovsky AS, Brody D, Bank I, Bhanji F. Pediatric resuscitation training-instruction all at once or spaced over time? Resuscitation. 2015;88:6–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Fischer MR, Kopp V, Holzer M, Ruderich F, Jünger J. A modified electronic key feature examination for undergraduate medical students: Validation threats and opportunities. Med Teach. 2005;27:450–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Hatala R, Norman G. Adapting the key features examination for a clinical clerkship. Med Educ. 2002;36:160–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Kopp V, Möltner A, Fischer MR. Key-feature problems for the assessment of procedural knowledge: A practice guide. GMS Z Medizinische Ausbildung. 2006;23:Doc50.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Page G, Bordage G, Allen T. Developing key-feature problems and examinations to assess clinical decision-making skills. Acad Med. 1995;70:194–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Nolan JP, Soar J, Zideman DA, Biarent D, Bossaert LL, Deakin C, Koster RW, Wyllie J, Böttiger B, on behalf of the ERC Guidelines Writing Group. European Resuscitation Council Guidelines for Resuscitation 2010. Section 1. Executive summary. Resuscitation. 2010;81:1219–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences. 2nd ed. Hillsdale: Erlbaum; 1988.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Benjamin AS, Tullis J. What makes distributed practice effective? Cogn Psychol. 2010;61:228–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable, all contributors qualify for (co-) authorship

Funding

No additional funding; investigator driven academic study supported exclusively by university resources.

Availability of data and materials

The dataset supporting the conclusions of this article is included within the Additional file 3: Table S2.

Authors’ contributions

JB designed the study, contributed to the production of videos and the finalisation of the key-feature test, contributed to data collection, and wrote the main parts of the manuscript. JRL contributed to the production of the videos, constructed and finalised the sequential questions of the video-based key-feature test, organised and equipped the testing sessions, collected and processed the data, and prepared a draft of the manuscript. JP constructed the technical platform of the electronic key-feature test, equipped the testing sessions and contributed to data collection. TS contributed to the study design and to the design of the emergency medicine course, and gave intellectual input to the final version of the manuscript. HG made important contributions to concept, interpretation of data, and the final manuscript. HP made important contributions to study design, literature search, and the final manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ information

JB is specialised in anaesthesiology and emergency medicine and in medical education and was consultant in anaesthesiology at Charité-Medical University Berlin. At present he is Head of Office at the Vice Deanery of Educational Affairs of the Faculty of Medicine, University of Zurich. JRL was a doctorate student at Charité-Medical University Berlin, and at present medical doctor. JP is scientific assistant at the department for eLearning of the Dieter Scheffner Centre for Medical Education at Charité-Universitätsmedizin Berlin. TS is a consultant in anaesthesiology and emergency medicine at Charité-Universitätsmedizin Berlin. HG is full professor at the Department of Educational Science, Faculty of Psychology, Educational Science and Sport Science, University of Regensburg (Germany) and at the Department of Teacher Education, Faculty of Education, University of Turku (Finland). HP is full professor and chair of the Dieter Scheffner Centre for Medical Education at Charité-Universitätsmedizin Berlin.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Consent for publication

Not applicable (No information was collected, which could have identified single students.)

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Students confirmed, that their anonymised data sets could be used for this study when entering the KF-test. No information was collected, which could have identified single students. The study was approved by the ethical committee of Charité-Universitätsmedizin Berlin (EA1/326/14) as indicated in the “methods” section.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jan Breckwoldt.

Additional files

Additional file 1: Table S1.

Student characteristics of sub-groups in massed course version. (DOCX 15 kb)

Additional file 2: Figure S1.

KF-test scores of sub-groups of the massed version. (DOCX 136 kb)

Additional file 3: Table S2.

Raw data (English titles). (XLS 305 kb)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Breckwoldt, J., Ludwig, J.R., Plener, J. et al. Differences in procedural knowledge after a “spaced” and a “massed” version of an intensive course in emergency medicine, investigating a very short spacing interval. BMC Med Educ 16, 249 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12909-016-0770-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12909-016-0770-6

Keywords